There are two purposes for assessing athletic performance. First, and more common, is to quantitatively determine improvements made following a training cycle. This allows the athlete and sports performance professional to examine if the training stimulus was sufficient to cause a positive adaptation. This method does not however, answer Bobbert’s third question, which asks, ‘Which changeable factors do you focus training on?’(Bobbert and Van Soest, 1994). Two athletes may perform the same during an assessment of 36.58 meter sprinting ability, but it only indicates they arrived at the same time and does not reflect how they traveled from start to finish. Therefore, a training cycle designed solely on a finish time will most likely include a plethora of arbitrarily chosen drills and exercises in an attempt to improve the outcome variable (36.58 meter time). Using this shotgun approach to program design will simply fail to focus on specific weaknesses and ultimately attenuate athletic development. The second purpose of athletic assessment is to pinpoint specific weaknesses in linear sprinting performance utilizing various splits. Determining splits requires the use of infrared timing sensors, whereby a gate is set at specified distances (in the current paper every 9.14 meters). This design allows for the collection of 9.14, 18.28, 27.42, and 36.58 meter times (cumulative splits) and also allows each 9.14 meter split to be examined independently (individual splits). To the author’s knowledge using split times to examine specific breakdowns in athletic performance and subsequently design a targeted training program focused on identified weaknesses is not common practice among sports performance professionals. A paradigm was designed using a sample of Division I female soccer and lacrosse players. Physical characteristics and 36.58 meter sprint times were similar between sports (Table 1). Based on 36.58 meter finish times the data was subsequently trisected (range = 0.41 seconds) to establish sub- groups, which were labelled above average (faster), average, and below average (slower). The rationale for creating these sub-groups was to determine if the paradigm could provide similar information within distinct classifications of performance. Statistical analysis showed significant differences between the sub-groups on the total finish time as well as each 9.14 meter split (faster < average < slower; Figure 1). Select individuals were chosen from each sub-group to demonstrate the appropriate use of split times. A range of 0.05 seconds was arbitrarily chosen since it was thought to represent nearly identical finish times for this sample of athletes. The athletes from the faster, average, and slower sub-groups had 36.58 meter times of 5.73 ± 0.02, 6.13 ± 0.02, and 6.27 ± 0.02 seconds, respectively. Simply examining these times would lead most to prescribe comparable training regimens within a particular sub-group, resulting in ‘faster’ athletes. This philosophy lacks depth and does not allow the sports performance professional to determine if there is a breakdown in acceleration or if metabolic inefficiency exists which prohibits maintenance of speed throughout the entire testing distance. The use of predetermined splits should provide some insight. Figures 2-4">4 display both cumulative (A) and individual (B) splits for the above average, average, and below average sub-group, respectively. Each line represents an individual athlete. Cumulative splits provide additional information regarding an athlete’s performance during a 36.58 meter sprint, however it remains difficult to pinpoint faults. For example, with the exception of one or two individuals, the lines all run parallel to one another and are closely related within a given sub-group. It does appear some separation exists for the first 9.14 meter cumulative split, which is more evident in the slow sub-group. However, not enough meaningful evidence exists to establish future training protocols which target specific areas of breakdown. Therefore, using cumulative splits remains a limiting factor for determining a specific training focus and does not clearly express strengths and weaknesses in linear sprinting performance. On the other hand, individual splits (Figs 2-4">4B) provide a drastically different picture compared to 36.58 meter time or cumulative splits. It becomes clearly evident how any given athlete travelled from start to finish. In fact, three distinct phases can be observed, which have been operationally defined as; initial acceleration (split 1), secondary acceleration (splits 2 and 3), and metabolic-stiffness transition (split 4). From a static start to 9.14 meters all athletes will increase velocity due to a rise in both stride length and frequency, hence the term initial acceleration. Schmolinsky (2000) clearly shows that between 10 and 30 meters velocity continues to rise, primarily due to an increase in stride length. However, the change in slope is not as steep compared to the initial acceleration (as reported by Schmolinsky (2000)) and therefore secondary acceleration is used to describe the combination of splits 2 and 3. Beyond 30 meters both stride length and frequency have reached a plateau (Schmolinsky, 2000) and so the ability to maintain speed beyond this distance will depend heavily on two factors: 1. anaerobic metabolism and 2. active muscular stiffness. Women tend to show a plateau in velocity sooner than men (Schmolinsky, 2000) and this could lead to a heightened reliability on anaerobic metabolism to maintain speed. Active muscular stiffness minimizes the vertical displacement of the center of mass (via knee and ankle stability) during the support phase (Kyrolainen et al., 1999). Research has shown women have 20-45% less stiffness compared to men during sprinting in conjunction with greater muscular activation (Granata et al., 2002a; 2002b). It is for these two reasons the phrase metabolic-stiffness transition is used to operationally define the final 9.14 meter split. It must be noted that while distinct phases of the 36.58 meter test have been identified in this paper, certain trainable aspects should not be considered specific to a particular phase, and will be interdependent with one another. Graphic representation of the individual split times associated with the three phases provides the necessary information to critique performance and create a focused training plan. For example, the 1st 9.14 meter split time (i.e., initial acceleration) can identify an individual that requires work in that area regardless of their total finishing time. In other words, one athlete considered fast and another considered slow could both have deficiencies in acceleration which require similar attention during a training cycle. In contrast, two athletes considered slow may require acceleration development, but show different faults for their poor start. One may need form and technique development while the other requires explosive power training. Clearly the data will not identify which factor is responsible for the deficit in acceleration. Visual assessment is a necessary component at this point to decipher which aspect should be the primary focus during the subsequent training cycle. An athlete that has technique flaws should develop the appropriate motor skills prior to initiating strength or power training. For example, if over-striding and heel strike occur during initial acceleration, specific instructions should be provided and drills performed to correct this particular deficiency. On the other hand, an individual who displays proper sprinting form will most likely benefit from strength and/or power development. Pinpointing a weakness and subsequently determining the cause will provide greater focus and heightened returns during training. For the purpose of this paper we have operationally defined secondary acceleration to indicate the distance between 9.14 and 27.42 meters (i.e., 2nd and 3rd splits). Because the 2nd and 3rd splits are an intermediate phase for this particular distance, determining the cause of a potential weakness in secondary acceleration may be difficult. Metabolic deficiencies could occur in this phase, however this is unlikely due to the fact that the duration from 9.14 to 27.42 meters is approximately 2 - 5 seconds. Nevertheless this cannot be ruled out as a cause since poor anaerobic power could reduce performance. Muscular power could also be a cause of poor performance in secondary acceleration. A recent study examining male field sport athletes reported that faster individuals showed shorter support (i.e., ground contact) phases (Murphy et al., 2003). Although not addressed in that investigation, it should be assumed that greater amounts of horizontal power were accomplished by the faster athletes since a shorter ground phase alone would not necessarily be beneficial to sprinting speed. Blazevich and Jenkins (2002) reported an improvement in 20 meter speed following seven weeks of training, but found no difference between groups when comparing high versus low velocity resistance training. So, while power production is important, the specific training stimulus for its development is unclear. Definitive answers will only be provided with further research. Finally, metabolic-stiffness transition, as we have operationally defined it, occurs during the last 9.14 meter split. The directionality of this particular segment indicates how an athlete finished during the 36.58 meter sprint. A line shifting to an upwards direction is shown for several individuals in each sub-group, which indicates a reduction in speed during the final 9.14 meters. While some might argue that athletes slow down prior to crossing the finish line, it was hypothesized that inadequate anaerobic metabolism is partly responsible for the decrease in speed. Additionally, the use of duplicate trials minimized any faulty interpretation of test scores due to an athlete simply slowing before the finish. Mathematical modeling of the world champion 100 meter sprint finals indicated that deceleration began after approximately six seconds of sprinting (Arsac and Locatelli, 2002). It should be expected that athletes of less caliber (e.g., college athletes) would begin to decelerate sooner and therefore rely to a greater extent on anaerobic metabolism to maintain speed. Pilot work from one of the authors (TB) has shown that an eight week program using interval conditioning improved the final 9.14 meter split time by an average of 0.11 ± 0.04 seconds for a group of high school female soccer players (unpublished data), suggesting a possible remedy for this particular weakness. Recent research has shown that short (< 10 seconds) sprints can alter enzymatic activity and improve 40 yard time after only six weeks (Dawson et al., 1998). This implies that a link exists between speed and speed endurance, and by improving metabolic efficiency an athlete can consequently impact, and in fact improve, speed (Matveyev, 1981). Complex motor skills, such as sprinting, may also rely on ankle and knee joint stiffness, but to what extent active muscular stiffness contributes to better performance is debatable (Granata et al., 2002a; 2002b; Kuitunen et al., 2002). Mechanical and neural properties responsible for the control of active muscle stiffness cannot be assessed using simple timing devices, but requires expensive laboratory equipment. Nonetheless, Hennessy and Kilty (2001) showed drop jump performance accounts for approximately 62% of the variance for 30 meter sprint time. Therefore, using drills with specific instructions to jump for maximal height or distance and require minimal contact with the ground should provide the necessary stimulus for improving active muscle stiffness and consequently sprinting ability. |