The results of this study are novel because they describe the nutrient intakes of athletes whose eating behaviors are “disordered” and compare the nutrient intakes of athletes whose disordered eating behaviors are primarily restrictive to those who engage in binge eating. Furthermore, this study included men athletes, who are often ignored in assessments of disordered eating patterns and nutrient intakes. The validity of these findings is strengthened by a large study population; the appropriate use of the DRIs to evaluate nutrient intakes; and the use of standardized diagnostic criteria rather than attitudinal criteria, such as body dissatisfaction, for the determination of disordered versus non-disordered eating. Although the high response rate for the women athletes (82%) is a strength of the study, the response rate for the men (54%) is a limitation. Disordered eating has been recognized by the ACSM as a problematic group of behaviors for some athletes, with potentially negative health consequences (Yeager et al., 1993; ACSM, 1997). The ACSM has defined disordered eating as a wide spectrum of harmful and often ineffective behaviors that athletes use to control their body weight. These behaviors include caloric restriction and a wide range of other behaviors, such as vomiting, diuretics, diet pills, laxatives, and fasting (Yeager et al., 1993; ACSM, 1997). There is little information available on the nature and prevalence of disordered eating in athletes, or on the nutritional consequences of these behaviors. Thus, the aims of this study were to assess the macro- and micronutrient intakes of men and women collegiate athletes with disordered eating behaviors and to compare athletes with restrictive- versus binge-eating behaviors. In general, we found that athletes with disordered eating patterns were not at increased risk for inadequate macro- or micronutrient intakes compared with asymptomatic athletes. To our knowledge, this is the first study that had as a primary aim to examine nutrient intakes in men athletes with disordered eating. There are several published reports on the nutrient intakes of men collegiate wrestlers-a population that is likely to have a high prevalence of disordered eating (Short and Short, 1983; Steen and McKinney, 1986). For example, in a study of 42 NCAA Division I wrestlers, Steen and McKinney reported that significant proportions of the athletes had intakes of energy, carbohydrate, vitamin A, vitamin B6, zinc, and magnesium that were less than two-thirds of the RDA. Large percentages of these athletes reported reducing food intake (81%), using saunas (51%), wearing a rubber suit while exercising (42%), or wrestling in a heated room (78%) to lose weight. However, it cannot be assumed that the group as a whole would be classified as having disordered eating patterns, and nutrient intakes were not compared between individuals who utilized disordered eating practices and those who did not. In our sample of men athletes that included individuals who reported restrictive- and binge-eating patterns, we found that absolute energy intakes were lower than recommended for all groups and that there were no significant differences among the groups. Similar to the results in the wrestlers (Steen and McKinney, 1986), athletes with disordered eating patterns were more likely to report restricting their intake of carbohydrate to control their weight. In addition, they consumed relatively less of their energy from carbohydrate than asymptomatic athletes. We also found that significant proportions of the men athletes had intakes of vitamin E (63%) and magnesium (59%) that were below the EARs. However, the frequencies of inadequate intakes were not greater in the athletes with disordered eating than in those who were asymptomatic. Interestingly, the disordered eating behaviors in the men athletes did not appear to be motivated by wanting to gain or lose weight, as the current BMI did not differ from the desired BMI for any of the groups. In contrast, the women athletes who reported disordered eating patterns wanted to lose significantly more weight than the asymptomatic athletes, although the desired BMIs did not differ among groups. Athletes with restrictive eating patterns were more likely to report limiting their intake of carbohydrate and using dietary supplements to control their weight than asymptomatic athletes. There were no significant differences in absolute or relative energy or macronutrient intakes among groups, but carbohydrate intakes for the disordered and asymptomatic athletes were below the recommendation for athletes. The two published reports of nutrient intakes in women athletes with disordered eating contain conflicting results. In a study of Norwegian elite women athletes, Sundgot-Borgen ((1993)) found no differences in absolute energy or macronutrient intake between athletes with anorexia athletica and controls, although both groups had intakes that were below the recommendations for energy and carbohydrate. This is in contrast to the results of Beals and Manore (1998), who reported significantly lower absolute energy, protein, and fat intakes in athletes with subclinical eating disorders compared to controls. There were no significant differences in mean micronutrient intakes among the disordered and asymptomatic groups in the current study. This result is consistent with those reported by Sundgot-Borgen (1993) and Beals and Manore (1998). We did not detect any significant differences in the frequencies of inadequate nutrient intakes using the EAR cut-point method between disordered and asymptomatic athletes. Beals and Manore reported that more athletes with subclinical eating disorders had intakes for calcium, iron, magnesium, zinc, niacin, vitamin B6, and vitamin B12 that were less than two-thirds of the RDA compared to control athletes. One potential explanation for the discrepant conclusions is that Beals reported only foods and not vitamin/mineral supplements, while the nutrient intakes presented in the current study include vitamin/mineral supplements. It is noteworthy that among women athletes who consumed alcohol, those with disordered eating had a significantly higher intake than the asymptomatic individuals. This finding is consistent with higher rates of alcohol use in individuals with eating disorders compared to their non-eating-disordered peers in non-athletic populations (Holderness et al., 1994). There are several possible reasons why we did not observe greater frequencies of nutritional inadequacies in athletes with disordered eating, particularly those with restrictive eating patterns, compared to asymptomatic athletes. First, by definition, individuals who are classified as symptomatic exhibit abnormal eating behaviors with less frequency and/or duration than individuals whose behaviors are consistent with clinical eating disorders. Thus, their disordered eating behavior may not be severe enough or may not occur with high enough frequency to affect their habitual nutrient intake. There were insufficient numbers of individuals who reported behaviors consistent with clinical eating disorders in the sample to compare nutrient intakes in clinical vs. subclinical eating disorders. Similarly, our ability to detect statistically significant differences between the groups was affected by the small numbers of athletes in the eating disorder groups. The final explanation relates to the methodological difficulties associated with assessment of usual nutrient intake. These include the ability of the respondents to recall what foods they ate and to accurately estimate serving size, limitations of the time period sampled being representative of habitual intake, alterations in the usual diet as a result of recording food intake, under-reporting of food intake, subject burden, and participant compliance. Given these difficulties, we chose to use a semi-quantitative food frequency questionnaire to assess habitual nutrient intake as opposed to written food records or a 24-hour dietary recall for several reasons. We wanted to maximize the response rate by increasing the likelihood that athletes would complete the dietary assessment. Our primary strategy for accomplishing this goal was to provide sufficient time for the athletes to complete the food frequency questionnaire in a team meeting, rather than relying on the athletes to complete and return food records. We were interested in habitual intake and because daily or weekly food intake can vary significantly, we chose not to use the 24-hour recall or written food records. Furthermore, as mentioned above, under-reporting is a recurrent problem in determining true food intake. Recently, energy expenditure assessed by doubly-labeled water has become the gold standard by which energy intake data are evaluated. Using this technique, the magnitude of the underestimation was ~15% using either a food frequency questionnaire or a seven-day written food record (Livingstone and Black, 2003). Thus, we were confident that the food frequency would perform as well as a written food record in this regard. Nevertheless, we recognize that food frequency questionnaires rely on self-reported data and are semi-quantitative. In addition, they may not accurately reflect nutrient intake in individuals with disordered eating patterns that are characterized by unstable eating habits, consumption of food in serving sizes that deviate significantly from normal portion sizes, or loss of some nutrients via purging. |