Research article - (2005)04, 543 - 549 |
A Comparison of Mental Strategies During Athletic Skills Performance |
Eugenio A. Peluso, Michael J. Ross, Jeffrey D. Gfeller, Donna J. LaVoie |
Key words: Sport psychology, performance enhancement techniques, self-talk, arousal regulation, mental imagery, attention |
Key Points |
|
|
|
Participants |
Participants were male (41; 27.3%) and female (109; 72.6%) college-aged student volunteers from introductory psychology classes. Age distribution of subjects was age 18 (n = 85), age 19 (n = 36), age 20 (n = 18), age 21 (n = 8), age 22 (n = 1), age 23 (n = 1) and age 25 (n = 1). The hours participating in organized athletics activity of subjects were 5.07 ± 7.02 hrs (ranging 0.00-40.00) per week. Participants were predominately right handed (138; 92%) and between the ages of 18 to 21. Individuals who participated in the study were issued extra-credit points to be used towards course grades. |
Equipment |
Participants were assessed using a regulation nine-foot Professional Golfer’s Association (P.G.A.) automatic putting mat, standard left and right handed thirty-five inch Tour Classic putters, and ten standard Top Flight XL golf balls. |
Sports imagery questionnaire |
Participants were instructed to fill out the Sports Imagery Questionnaire ( The five subscales of the SIQ have been shown to have a favorable internal consistency. A Cronbach’s alpha analysis on the five imagery subscales reported an internal consistency ranging from .70 for Motivational General-Arousal to .88 for Motivational Specific (Hall et al., |
Demographics questionnaire |
Participants were asked to complete a self-report questionnaire in order to attain demographic information (e.g., age, gender, etc.) as well as to assess their athletic/golfing activity and perceived levels of athletic/golfing abilities. |
Self-Efficacy Questionnaire |
Participants were asked to complete a self-report questionnaire in order to attain an estimate on how many putts out of ten one believed he or she would make. |
Validity check questionnaire |
Finally, a post-experiment questionnaire was issued to all participants to determine if the participants engaged in any PET practice during the treatment trials. If participants in the delayed or control conditions affirm the use of simultaneous self-talk or imagery practice during the posttest evaluation, his or her data did not undergo further analyses. In addition, participant’s data sets from the internally verbalized conditions who denied the use of their respective mental set during the posttest evaluation, did not undergo further analyses. |
Procedure |
Conditions for participation followed APA ethical standards. Participants were told the nature of the study and were given an opportunity to withdraw from the study at any time without the loss of any extra-credit points. To maintain confidentiality, names of the participants were not placed on the data sheets. Participants were randomly assigned to one of nine groups; a simultaneous externally verbalized self-talk or imagery condition, a delayed externally verbalized self-talk or imagery condition, a simultaneous internally verbalized self-talk or imagery condition, a delayed internally verbalized self-talk or imagery condition, and a no instruction control condition. All participants were asked to perform a golf-putting task. The experimenter initially demonstrated twice how and where to putt the golf balls. Participants performed a pretest trial during which the participants were instructed to putt ten golf balls from six feet away. Pretest trials were conducted before any PET’s were introduced to the participants. Participants were assessed for the number of correct shots made into the hole. Between trials, during the posttest interventions, participants were asked to close their eyes and the experimenter instructed the participants to visualize themselves performing the same putt successfully, but also instructed them to do so with their appropriate mental set, (i.e., “aim, back, birdie”) or imagery vignette. Participants were instructed to say “aim ”when they are measuring up their shot, “back ”when they initiated their back swing, and “birdie ”when the ball was struck. Participants in the externally verbalized conditions vocalized their self-talk mental set or imagery vignette out loud at a level in which the experimenter could hear clearly; whereas participants in the internally verbalized conditions vocalized their self-talk mental set or imagery vignette silently to themselves. Participants in the simultaneous self-talk conditions performed their putts while vocalizing either their self-talk mental set, (i.e., “aim, back, birdie”) or imagery vignette. Participants in the delayed self-talk conditions were asked to focus on their self-talk mental set, (i.e., “aim, back, birdie”) or imagery vignette prior to any putting attempts. Between trials, participants were asked to engage in their specific PET for an additional minute. Participants in the no instruction control condition were asked to attempt all of their putts without any PET’s. Between trails, participants were asked to count out loud backwards from 500 to zero, by threes. The introduction of the counting acted as a distracting task for the participant to reduce the likelihood of the participant engaging in any self-initiated self-talk and/or imagery techniques. At the conclusion of the pretest and posttest evaluations, participants were asked if they engaged in any type of self-talk or imagery practice during the experiment. Next, participants filled out the Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, which assessed how many putts a participant believed he or she would make. Finally, all participants filled out the Sports Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) in order to determine their respective imagery skills and abilities. Each participant attempted twenty putts: ten during the pretest evaluation and ten putts during the posttest evaluation. Participants were assessed for the number of correct putts into the hole. Upon the conclusion of the posttest evaluation, mean scores were calculated and difference scores from the pretest and posttest trials were compared. |
|
|
The majority of participants reported playing organized athletics (e.g., high school, 88%; college, 19.3%; intramurals, 53.3%) and a minority of the overall population reported playing organized golf (e.g., high school, 4.7%; college <1%; intramurals; 2%). A series of Pearson’s correlations examined the relationship between participants’ overall time spent playing regulation P.G.A. and miniature golf and overall difference scores. Results indicated a significant relationship between P.G.A. golf experience and overall difference scores across all conditions r = 0.172; p < 0.05. Follow-up analyses suggested that of the 68% of the participants who selected “other ”on the demographic questionnaire, approximately 87% reported “never ”playing golf; therefore suggesting an overall unfamiliar/novice sample. In addition, results indicated no significant relationship between miniature golf experience and overall difference scores r = 0.044; p > 0.05 across conditions. A series of ANOVAs were conducted across all nine conditions comparing pretest putting abilities. Results indicated that no significant differences were seen across all conditions across pre-test putting trials; therefore suggesting that participants’ overall putting abilities were commensurate F (8, 141) = 5.779, p > 0.05. A 9 x 2 repeated measures ANOVA was conducted across all nine conditions comparing overall outcome performance between participants’ pretest and posttest trial scores. Results indicated no significant differences across conditions and between trials existed, F (8, 141) = 1.916, p > 0.05. In contrast, a follow-up ANOVA indicated a significant interaction across conditions between participants’ overall difference score across pretest and posttest trials, F (8, 141) = 4.009, p < 0.05. Follow up paired comparisons on overall difference scores indicated improved putting performance across seven of the eight PET experimental conditions. Specifically, participants in the simultaneous internal imagery condition exhibited the largest difference score, Cohen’s d = .7359, p< .05. In addition, results indicated a negative decline in performance across overall differences scores for participants in the delayed external imagery and no instruction control group (please refer to Given the high degree of variability regarding the number of hours of organized athletic activity participants reported, a series of one-way ANOVAs comparing performance across conditions was conducted. Results indicated that participants who endorsed ten hours or less a week of athletic activity preferred self talk interventions over the imagery and the no instruction control condition F (2, 119) = 4.389, p < 0.05. In contrast, participants who endorsed ten hours or more a week of athletic activity preferred imagery strategies to self-talk and the no instruction control condition F (2, 25) = 5.27, p < 0. 05. In addition, no significant differences were found between participants in both the ten hour or more and ten hour and less condition when assessing for preferences styles between internalized, externalized, simultaneous, and delayed conditions. These findings are consistent with the current literature which states that novice athletes will often engage in self-talk practice for athletic skill mastery whereas more experienced athletes will implement imagery techniques as an arousal regulation and/or motivational technique (Cox, |
|
|
Results of the current study suggest that participants who engaged in several performance enhancement techniques exhibited enhanced performance on a golf putting task when compared to participants in a control condition. Overall, both self-talk and imagery conditions were found to significantly increase putting performance. Internalized forms of self-talk and imagery yielded higher differences scores when compared to external and the no-instruction control conditions. In addition, participants who endorsed limited athletic familiarity and activity (e.g., ten hours or less) preferred self-talk practice whereas participants who endorsed higher ratings scores of athletic familiarity and activity (e.g., ten hours or more) preferred imagery strategies. Regardless of skill level or type of sport, PET’s (e.g., self-talk, imagery) allows coaches, trainers, athletes, and sport psychologists seemingly limitless opportunities to design treatment plans, which focus exclusively on an athlete’s intellectual, emotional, and physical strengths and weaknesses. For instance, in a series of case studies with elite junior tennis players who implemented a year long mental training program (e.g., goal-setting, positive thinking/self-talk, concentration/routines, arousal regulation, and imagery). Mamassis and Doganis ( In contrast, researchers are suggesting that directly applying self-talk strategies in athletic competition may serve as a potential problem for athletes. For instance, Ming and Martin ( Various situational elements of team athletics may not allow an athlete enough time to engage in appropriate self-talk practice. In light of these potential problems, it is suggested that self-talk interventions may be best implemented with athletes who are engaging in expected individualized activities (i.e., free throw shooting, golf-putting, and field goal kicking). Seeing both the positive and potential negative factors associated with self-talk interventions, researchers have agreed that the primary concern regarding self-talk is the need to adopt more systematic research paradigms (Theodorakis et al., |
Conclusions |
A potential limitation of this study may be the subject sample pool. Saint Louis University is a traditional midwestern, Jesuit institution, which is primarily Caucasian, therefore not truly representing the current demographics of the general population. In addition, the results of this study may be difficult to generalize across a number of athletic situations. For instance, participants completed this study in an indoor laboratory setting using an artificial putting mat; therefore, the final results may have varied if participants completed the study on an outdoor putting green. Also, participants completed the study on an individual basis and were unaware of the results of other participants. Participants were not issued any incentives for superior performance and were given as much time as needed to attempt their putts. Given these factors, the results of this study may have varied across conditions if participants were placed in a more competitive format. In addition, participants were only assessed on putting ability; therefore one should not generalize these results to other golf skills (e.g., chipping, bunker shots, fairway shots, driving off the tee), athletic ability (e.g., novice, advanced, elite), and other sports. Furthermore, outcome performance was solely determined by the number of putts made and not by other factors such as closeness to the hole or number of attempts to make a putt. Finally, participant’s imagery styles and abilities may have influenced posttest scores. In order to control for these effects, participants were asked to complete the Sports Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ; Hall et al., In light of these limitations, several areas of future research are suggested. First, these findings should be replicated with participants representing different levels of skills (i.e., professional, collegiate, novice) and types of sports (i.e., team versus individual). Additionally, it is suggested that further research include the investigation of the effects of simultaneous, externally verbalized, task-relevant imagery techniques in comparison to more traditional forms of imagery practice (i.e., premeditation, relaxation). Finally, future research is needed to compare the effectiveness of visually presented imagery interventions (i.e., watching a video of successful golf putts, live modeling) with verbally instructed imagery interventions. |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
I would like to extend my warmest thanks to my graduate school advisor and mentor, Dr. Michael J. Ross. His guidance and support has made this research project worthwhile. I would also like to thank Dr. Jeffrey D. Gfeller, Dr. Donna J. LaVoie, and my research assistants for their suggestions and assistance on this project. Finally, I would like to recognize the Creighton University psychology faculty, especially Dr. Mark E. Ware, Dr. Amy Badura, and Dr. Ramie Cooney for their collective mentorship and contributions on this project. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|