Combat Sports Special Issue 1, Research article - (2006)05, 60 - 73 |
How Boxers Decide to Punch a Target: Emergent Behaviour in Nonlinear Dynamical Movement Systems |
Robert Hristovski1,, Keith Davids2, Duarte Araújo3, Chris Button4 |
Key words: Martial arts, boxing, affordances, decision-making, action selection |
Key Points |
|
Informational Constraints in Boxing: What sources of information do boxers use to coordinate their actions towards a target? |
In combat sports learners need to become attuned to relevant properties of the environment that produce unique patterns of information flows (e.g., optical information from a punch bag or opponent's body). Such flow patterns can act as invariant information sources to constrain decision-making on the type of attack to be made (e.g., upper cut or jab). During training boxers learn to couple their movements to these critical information sources when selecting appropriate punches. Because of the time constraints on action, boxers need to narrow down the minimal information needed to select a stroke and to regulate their movements from the enormous amount available in the performance environment. In the sports sciences' literature there have been no previous attempts to study the perceptual variables that might act as informational constraints as boxers punch targets during practice. However, previous motor development and motor control research on interceptive actions point to some possible candidate variables. One candidate variable that contains relevant information for generating specific boxing actions could be based on information used to perceive a target's 'reachability'. Reachability has been shown to be perceived very early in infant development (Rochat, From this theoretical perspective, there were two main aims of the present study. First, we sought to identify some of the key informational constraints from the whole set that boxers might use to generate punching actions in a tactical manner. During training sessions, boxers explore and discover the most adaptive relations between the informational sources and their actions. Coaching literature from the martial arts in general suggests that one of the most important constraints regulating the punches of fighters is the perception of scaled distance to the opponent (e.g., Walker, Second, we believed that this type of informational constraint might interact with other constraints used by boxers to shape a kind of 'perceptual-motor landscape' of emergent actions based on their perceived efficiency of punches. We expected that the relations between intentionality and distance to target information would mould this perceptual-motor landscape in boxers as they selected the most efficient punches at specific locations. In this study we manipulated performer-target distances to understand effects of perceived striking efficiency in boxing, allowing us to plot the dynamic probability landscape of striking patterns that might emerge in boxers. During the task of punching a heavy bag, six classical types of boxing action patterns were under consideration: right jab, left jab, right hook, left hook, right uppercut and left uppercut. Our hypothesis was that the initial activation of a punching action, its probability of occurrence and its disappearance would all exhibit dependence on the distance to the target and on perceived striking efficiency by boxers. |
|
|
To achieve these empirical aims, 8 novice boxers, who had just completed a two-semester course of elementary boxing techniques, aged 21-23 years, were required to select appropriate punches to a black leather hanging heavy bag fixed to the wall with its bottom 95 cm from the floor. Two boxers reported weak left handedness and all other participants reported being right handed. Participants were not given prescriptive instructions on punches to be selected but were required to maximize the efficiency and the diversity of their punching actions. The boxers also complied with a number of other instructional constraints including the requirement to perform sequences of strikes in a parallel stance with their toes aligned with different distance markers from the target. Before starting the activity, performers were stringently reminded not to overbalance which is a prerequisite for a successful punch (Walker, In our analysis the following measures were recorded: (i) scaled boxer - target distances D determined as a ratio between the physical distance of the participants from the target (X) and performers' arm length (L) i.e. D = X/L. Actually, the reachability condition can be formally expressed as R = X/( Leff + Llean) ≤ 1, or as a scaled distance (ratio) between the objective physical target - participant distance X and the sum of the effective length of the arm (Leff) and the lean length (Llean). The effective length of the arm depends on the degree of flexion in the elbow joint. The larger the angle of flexion (as in hooks and uppercuts with respect to the jab strokes) the smaller is effective arm length. Llean = X - X1, where X1 is the distance measured from the target to the projection of the participant's active arm acromion process on the floor. If the forward lean leads the projection of the acromion to pass over the X position, which is a usual consequence of a forward lean, then a positive value of effective arm length will result. During participants' activity, due to the non-rigid instructional constraints Leff, Llean, and consequently reachability, R values were strongly fluctuating quantities and thus less suitable as stable distance measures. (ii) the absolute probabilities of occurrence of action patterns i.e. relative frequencies p(k(D)) = k(D)/n, where k(D) is the number of occurrences of a certain type of punch at each scaled boxer - target distance (D) and n is the constant length of the sequence (n = 60); (iii) the mean group probabilities Pi of each of the three types of punches (jabs, hooks and uppercuts) at each scaled boxer - target distance (D): Pi(D) = 1 - Pj (D) + Pk(D), where Pi(D), Pj (D) and Pk (D) are the mean group probabilities of the three types of punches; (iv) mean group perceived punching efficiencies for each type of boxing shot (jab, hook and upper cut), at each scaled boxer - target distance: (E1,…,E3 (D)); (v) unpredictability of punching activity by the participants was assessed by the Shannon entropy measure: H(p(D)) = -Σi Pi(D)ln Pi(D); for (i =1,…,j; in our case j = 6, equal to the number of punching actions), i.e. as average information over all outcomes in the sequence; (vi) diversity was assessed by the symmetry measure S(p(D)) = 1 - (1/N) Σi |∆p(D)|i, for (i = 1,…,N), where N is the number of combinations of class 2 for a set of 6 elements (6 types of strokes), and | ∆p(D)|i is the absolute value of the differences between the probabilities of occurrence of each action pattern. In this study, for 6 possible types of strokes, there were N = 15 probability differences to calculate for each sequence per participant. (vii) efficiency ratio was calculated as E = Σi Ei (D) /Σi |∆E|i (D), for (i = 1…N), where N is the number of general types of actions (jabs, hooks and uppercuts) and the number of differences between the perceived punching efficiencies, making N=3; Ei (D) are the scaled distance (D) dependent mean group perceived punching efficiencies and |∆E|i(D) are the absolute values of the D dependent mean group perceived punching efficiency differences ( |∆E|1 = |Ejabs - Ehooks|; |∆E|2 = | Ejabs - Euppercuts|; |∆E|3 = | Ehooks - Euppercuts |). E measure increases if the sum of the perceived efficiencies increases and the sum of the perceived efficiency differences decreases. The group mean probability differences for the action modes were calculated as: |
Results and Discussion |
Distance affordances |
From the data several characteristics concerning decision-making behaviour in boxing are readily apparent. In particular, it was noted that specific boxing action patterns emerged as affordances of the individually-scaled distance to the target. We observed significant differences between the bounded regions of scaled distances for first time appearances and disappearances of specific boxing action patterns in the boxers. A one-way repeated measures analysis of variance revealed a significant distance main effect for the initial emergence scaled distances of the boxing action modes F(5,35) = 44.58; p < 0.00001. Kolmogorov-Smirnov D statistics of the critical scaled distance distributions for the first time activation of the whole set of the boxing action modes were not significant (p > 0.20). In similar vein Lilliefors probabilities (p > 0.20) as well as Shapiro -Wilk W statistics (all larger than p < 0.62) showed no significant departure from normal distribution. In almost all comparisons of distances we obtained significant differences (see Even the more stringent criteria such as Bonferroni corrections for alpha levels preserved these distance effects. In similar vein, boxer - target distance dependency was revealed by a repeated - measures analysis of variance on the distances at which disappearance of previously formed action patterns occurred F(3,21) = 2467.93; p < 0.00001). Post - hoc tests yielded significant scaled distance differences between the annihilation of the jab (180, -180) and uppercut (90yz, -90yz) punching activity (see Even the more stringent criteria such as Bonferroni corrections for alpha levels preserved these distance effects. In similar vein, boxer - target distance dependency was revealed by a repeated -measures analysis of variance on the distances at which disappearance of previously formed action patterns occurred F(3,21) = 2467.93; p < 0.00001). Post - hoc tests yielded significant scaled distance differences between the annihilation of the jab (180, -180) and uppercut (90yz, -90yz) punching activity (see These results corroborated our expectations that different action patterns would disappear at different scaled boxer - target distance bounded areas. Overall, boxers revealed the following behavior: At scaled distances D > 1.2 the only observed state for the performers was the guard (i.e. the quiescent) state (0 deg.). At scaled distances of D around 1.2 the previously quiescent jab modes of action (180 deg. and -180 deg.) emerged. We observed that, in all performers, right and left jab action modes emerged for the same individual values of D (see The obtained Pearson product - moment correlation coefficient r = 1.00 between the right and left jab initial action emergence corroborated this finding. For values around D = 0.9 the hooks (90xz deg. and -90xz deg.) action modes emerged. In only one out of eight performers the hook patterns emerged at different values of the scaled distance D. All of the other performers activated the hook patterns at the same individual D values. This finding was verified by the significant Pearson product - moment correlation coefficient between the right and left hook action modes r = 0.86; p = 0.006. Interestingly, uppercut action patterns did not show this kind of relationship. Right uppercut action modes emerged around greater D values (D = 0.8) than the left uppercuts (D = 0.65). The Pearson product - moment correlation coefficient between the right and left uppercut first time emergence D values was r = - 0.09; p = 0.826. This result signified that the initial emergence of uppercuts was the least coupled of events to scaled distance information. Taking into account that left and right jabs as well as left and right hooks emerged for the first time at the same individual scaled distances D, which was not the case for the left and right uppercuts, it can be assumed that scaled distances at which left and right uppercuts emerged for the first time might reveal the specific boxing handedness of the performers. Generally, the full set of boxing actions was active in the region 0.65 < D <0. 45. At the scaled distance D = 0.45 a spontaneous disappearance of the jab action patterns occurred. The Pearson product - moment correlation coefficient between the right and left jabs r = 1.00 confirmed that this disappearance occurred for the same individual D values (see These data suggested that decisions emerged as boxers picked up and exploited information about specific types of actions afforded at specific distances scaled for each individual. Significant differences between the mean group relative distances of the initial activation of different action patterns depended on the interaction between environmental constraints (absolute distance from the target) and the structural anatomical constraints of each individual boxer (effective length of upper limbs). The initiation of jab punches enabled the use of full arm length, making the effective limb length equal to its real length in extension, whereas hooks and uppercuts, depending on the degree of flexion in the elbow joint, always had a shorter effective length at the moment of collision with the target. Consequently different critical relative distances for the emergence of different action patterns were identified in the experiment. In other words, changes to the perceived context led the motor system of each boxer into and out of different functional coordination states. This feature of performance is depicted in The results support the expressed view that, in picking up affordances of target strikeability, information on perceived reachability to a target plays a crucial role. Hence, it could be tentatively assumed that the perceived strikeability by participants is merely a special case of this early acquired affordance reported in the motor development literature. However, our data revealed that reachability is not solely responsible for the observed decision-making behaviour of the boxers. First, at the point of jab annihilation the target was reachable, although at that very point jabs ceased to be active. Second, at the point of disengagement of uppercuts the target was also reachable, although uppercut actions were being annihilated at this point. These data imply that besides reachability, other performer and task constraints, perhaps specifically related to the intended intensity of collision between the fists and target, are considered by boxers for the target to be perceived as strikeable. From a constraints-led perspective, these data confirm that handstrike selection is an emergent phenomenon dependent on the continuing interaction of intentionality, perceptual information and body-scaled affordances for each individual boxer (see Newell, |
Movement-information coupling |
As reported in Similarly, Significant partial correlations between the probabilities of occurrence of action patterns and perceptual judgments of their efficiency suggest that the dynamic perceptual-motor landscape formed by boxers during performance is particularly sensitive to the perception of punching efficiency. The positive and negative influences of perceived efficiencies exemplify how the distance-dependent reciprocal enhancements and suppressions of action patterns were shaping the perceptual-motor landscape of individual boxers (see |
Punching unpredictability, diversity and efficiency ratio dependencies |
Pearson product - moment correlation coefficients between the scaled distance dependent unpredictability as measured by entropy (H), diversity as measured by symmetry (S) and efficiency ratio (E) are presented in These three measures showed significant positive relationships. Change of group mean punching unpredictability and diversity as a function of the performer - target scaled distance is presented in The maximum of perceived efficiency ratio does not imply that all of the action patterns attained maximal perceived efficiency values. For example hooks and uppercuts possessed larger perceived efficiency values for smaller scaled boxer - target distances than D = 0.6. Jabs attained maximal perceived efficiency values for somewhat larger scaled distance values. The maximum of the perceived efficiency ratio signified that there is an optimal proportion between the perceived efficiencies of the actions and their differences. The maximum of this function is attained when there are relatively high perceived efficiencies of the whole set of actions and small inter-efficiency differences. In other words, in this region the sum of the perceived punching efficiencies is relatively high and the perceived punching efficiency differences are minimal. This proportion maximizes the efficiency ratio function. On the other hand the unpredictability and diversity measures increase when the probabilities of all action patterns attain closer values. From the previously discussed results it was clear that in this specific region all types of punching action patterns were active and the probabilities of their occurrence possessed relatively similar values. This characteristic maximizes the unpredictability and diversity measures as well. Now starting from D = 0.6 and moving toward the larger or lower D values (see Overall, the relations between handstriking unpredictability (H), diversity (S) and efficiency ratio (E) show that the performers were able to discover and exploit the scaled distances D where the perceived efficiency differences of their action patterns were minimal and yet their absolute values were relatively high making the efficiency ratio (E) maximal. The observed relationships between the perceived efficiency ratio and the action unpredictability and diversity is yet another corroboration of the sensitivity of human movement systems (represented here by the novice boxers) to the interacting environmental and individual information flows which constrain their actions toward a target. |
Movement selection meta-stability and diversity |
The meta-stability (dynamics at the edge of instability) evident in the boxers' punching data is portrayed in For fixed values of scaled distance to the target there are coexisting (simultaneously available) states of actions implying that the dynamics of boxers' movement systems over time is dominated by transitions between different regions of the perceptual- motor landscape. To enable the flexible switching between the punching modes the boxers' movement system has to be poised at the edge of instability from where each punching mode can be spontaneously activated under the task and the perceived environmental constraints. It is important to note first that through a systematic gradual change of the scaled boxer - target distance, the number of available movement solutions changed abruptly. The probability of these abrupt changes (i.e. bifurcations) of specific punching action modes increased in well defined D regions, i.e. when the perceptual - motor system of the boxers became maximally susceptible to the small changes of the scaled distances, which together showed that these changes were most probably due to nonlinear dynamical causes. As presented on The meta-stability (weak instability) of punch selection is a key characteristic of the action flexibility of boxers because it avoids maladaptive mode-locked (more rigid) forms of behaviour that are too stable (and thus predictable) in the dynamic context of the martial arts. This adaptive behavior enables boxers to increase the uncertainty of their actions with a maximum occurring in the region around D = 0.6 that corresponds to the maximum values of the unpredictability diversity and efficiency ratio measures (see |
Emergence of performer - performer coordinative states |
Strategic positions of boxers may be observed as performer - performer coordination dynamical states similar to those observed during sub-phases of sports such as dribbling in basketball and sailing regattas (see Araújo et al., Under the task constraints of boxing, on the one hand opponents are attracted towards one another due to the main goal of the task (to strike each other), whereas on the other, the region of striking unpredictability, peaking at D = 0.6, acts as a repeller, driving opponents towards its minima D = 1.2 and D = 0. Through competition and cooperation of these contrary tendencies relatively stable regions are formed around the unpredictability and efficiency ratio minima. Data from this study supported anecdotal evidence that those minima are located at the edge of jab reachability and fluctuate so that the jabs could emerge. The incursions of the attacker (boxer A in |
Conclusions |
The data from this study have shown how a constraints-led framework on movement behaviour emphasizes the importance of the environmental, task and individual constraints on human movement systems. The results of this investigation on decision-making in boxers permit a number of practical conclusions to be drawn, as outlined below: The results obtained in this study demonstrated that dynamical systems theory could provide a useful and relevant theoretical framework for investigating decision making behaviour in sports such as boxing, as exemplified by the task of punching a heavybag. Interesting questions are raised over the differences and similarities of dynamical properties amongst a range of combat sports such as boxing, karate and kickboxing. One should expect that a variety of dynamical differences exist due to the differences in the number of limbs involved, the inter- and intra-limb coordination states that are enabled by rules and permissible actions and differences in limb lengths of participants. It is expected that differences in all of the measures and their interdependencies used in this investigation exist which would also change the properties of performer - performer coordination. These issues should be addressed in further investigations of combat sports motivated by dynamical systems theory. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|