Effective instruction may be crucial to the pursuit of optimal sporting performance. The most significant role of the physical education teacher or the coach is to give information about the skills’ execution in the form of feedback (Hodges and Franks, 2002) and has been found to be a key tool in improving and learning motor skills (Schmidt and Wrisberg, 2004). According to the cognitive approach the role of instructions and criticism on performance is a crucial factor for learning (Wulf and Shea, 2004) however, the ecological (Gibson, 1979) and dynamical systems approach (Kelso, 1981; Stergiou, Harbourne and Cavanaugh, 2006) of performance and learning support that information about the movement from an external source feedback is a second order constraint. Children begin to form impressions about their own self-worth based on the types of experiences they have and the nature of the feedback they get about their performance. If children are to feel competent, teachers must give them appropriate information about their performance. It is not enough merely to praise them for trying. Teachers must be selective in providing reinforcement and be certain that the behaviour of a child is appropriate for a particular reinforcement. Many researchers attempted to find the most appropriate methods of providing information through feedback to refine and develop motor skills (Salmoni et al., 1984). It is important to realize that this information can be acquired through many different methods, not all of which are as effective as each other (Amorose and Weiss, 1998; Williams and Hodges, 2005). Providing feedback in the form of verbal cueing facilitates the performance of the task by verbally indicating vital form characteristics (Landin, 1996), enhances attention and provides additional information that may not be available through visual observation. (Janelle et al., 2003). Lee et al., 1993 suggested that instructors’ feedback is typically verbal and in the form of positive, non-specific evaluative statements. Providing verbal cues about errors and corrections is useful for learners especially for the beginners (Kernodle and Carlton, 1992). The provision of encouragement with feedback that will help improve a skill (corrective feedback or criticism) may help the child improve and also believe the idea that the child can do better and improve the self confidence. However, less is known about the effectiveness of feedback in skills of different difficulty level (More and Franks, 1996; Hughes and Franks, 1997). Kernodle and Carlton, 1992 supported that when feedback provided contains error and correction cues is more useful for the difficult tasks. Schmidt and Lee, 1998 proposed that more research is needed for examining the relation of skills’ difficulty with feedback effectiveness. A mediating factor between the presentation of the instructions by the coach and the performance of the skill by the player might be the cognitive process of self confidence (Escarti and Guzman, 1999). The most powerful source of self-confidence is mastery of a skill (Bandura, 1977; 1997; Harter, 1978; Vealey et al., 1998). Allen and Howe, 1998 showed that self-confidence of the athletes is determined by coach feedback. Feedback can be accepted as a reward when there is a correct execution and this may increase self confidence levels or as a criticism when errors of the execution are corrected and the level of self confidence is decreased. Smith et al., 1995 asserted that feedback either in the form of error correction or in the form of praise and criticism can have a significant effect on young athletes’ psychology and self confidence. Feltz, 1988 argued that it is still not clear whether the guidance and feedback of a coach can have an effect on an athlete’s self-confidence. Lyster and Ranta, 1997 attempted to find what types of error treatments encourage learners and the results have been found to be quite complicated. Research on instructor feedback and student uptake does not yield conclusive claims and more research is needed. Many researchers investigated different methods of instruction that improve learning in laboratory settings (Vickers et al., 1999) in classroom settings (Scheeler and Lee, 2002) or in applied settings (Goode and Magill, 1986) However, there is not much empirical evidence for the effect of instructors’ corrective feedback on skills’ learning and self confidence for different type of skills (Franks, 1997; Hughes and Franks, 1997; More and Franks, 1996) including sport related cognitive complex solving tasks (McCullagh and Little, 1990; Sanchez and Bambouras, 2006; Silverman, 1994). Rink et al., 1996 proposed that the lack of empirical evidence to support any one approach to the teaching-learning process over another precludes the efficacy of suggesting a ‘model’ profile for coaches’ pedagogical content interventions. It is important for coaches to know how these different sources of feedback work both alone and in conjunction with other instructional techniques to improve learning of different complexity skills. Scheeler et al., 2004 mentioned that determining and interpreting the impact that different types of feedback have on performance has been difficult because there are number of complex theoretical processes of mechanisms involved. Williams and Hodges (2005) added that it might need further study before a complete understanding of its nature and significance is possible. Bunker, 1991 suggested that children acquire self-confidence and self-esteem as a result of successful experience. This experiment was designed to investigate how different types of corrective feedback (positive and correction cues, error cues or a combination) improve learning of a skill and alter self confidence of the participants. An additional purpose was to investigate whether these corrective feedback methods have the same effects in easy or difficult skills. In previous experiments (Tzetzis and Votsis, 2006), the effect of feedback may have been confounded with the level of difficulty of the skills, thus this experiment manipulates the level of difficulty to see whether feedback differentially affects outcome and self confidence when the skill difficulty varies. The purpose was to investigate the effect of three different treatment conditions (corrective feedback), the effect of time and the effect of difficulty level as well as their interaction on retention of the outcome and self-confidence, of two badminton skills with different difficulty level, for youth participants. The relationship of the participants’ self confidence by their outcome scores was also investigated. Since participants are novices and corrective feedback as well as error identification feedback are both necessary in directing, correcting and motivating them in practice, it was hypothesized that the combined method of positive feedback and instructional cues on execution as well as error cues would have the best results, improving the outcome and self confidence scores of both skills, across time. |