The aim of the present study was to examine the differences in game-related statistics between basketball starters and nonstarters players when related to game outcome and team quality in women’s basketball. It was argued that the gender of the players and their physical differences would configure different game-related statistical profile. There are rule differences between the sample studied (WNBA) and the European basketball. In particular, Reimer, 2005 compared FIBA and North-American rules, and found the following differences for WNBA 2005 season: i) two time periods of 20 minutes instead of four periods of 10 minutes; ii) thirty seconds in each ball possession instead of twenty four seconds; iii) six fouls per player instead of five personal fouls; and iv) seven team fouls to go to the free-throw line instead of five team fouls. Therefore, despite the present results are characterizing the highest level of women’s competition; extrapolation to other contexts should consider differences in rules. The results identified several differences from those obtained in men’s players’ available research (see Sampaio et al., 2006a). Results from the discriminant analysis showed the power of successful 2-point field-goal, successful free-throws and assists discriminating between starters and nonstarters players in all four analyses. These variables are offence-related, and reflect the importance of field-goal selection after good passes, or getting to the free-throws line as often as possible with better decision-making and game situations that allow receiving an opponent’s personal foul with free-throws. In women’s basketball the importance of successful 2-point field-goals and assists are strongly associated with winning teams (Gómez et al., 2006) and are the basis of offensive efficiency reflecting that best teams are tactically disciplined with clearly assigned responsibilities. Specifically, Oliver, 2004 argued that in the WNBA the game pace has been getting slower, and efficiency has been getting better, this may be explained because coaches prepare games and competitions with more emphasis on better field-goals selection and better ball-handling skills. The fact that starters made more 2-point field-goals and assists suggests, on the one hand, that starters exhibit better psychological responses such as greater role efficiency and desire for affiliation that allow them to get better team coordination and, consequently, field-goals opportunities (Bray, 2002; Gruber and Gray, 1982). And, on the other hand, women’s players are much more likely than men’s players to be disturbed by negative comments or crowd support (Pendleton, 2001) then it is not surprising that the nonstarters missed more 2-point field-goals and failed more passes. This could reflect that nonstarters are less task conscious and less confident with their own performance than starters (Gruber and Gray, 1982), as a consequence of that they get poorer offensive performances. The importance of successful-free-throws has been found in men’s teams discriminating team success (Ibáñez et al., 2003; Kozar et al., 1994), whereas in women’s teams did not discriminate winning and losing teams (Gómez et al., 2006). This game-related statistic discriminated starters and nonstarters, and may represent the importance of penetrating the opponents defence, because it increases high field-goals percentages and personal fouls received (Trninic et al., 2002). The differences in free-throws performance between starters and nonstarters may be associated to: i) technical performances and psychological aspects (i.e., concentration, motivation, confidence or task consciousness) that affect the free-throws execution; and ii) the offensive production, where starters generate better field-goal situations and get to draw fouls. In both groups (best and worst teams) the remaining variables that best discriminated between starters and nonstarters were defence-related. In best teams, both groups were discriminated by defensive rebounds and fouls, these variables were found in men’s teams as the most powerful variables discriminating starters and nonstarters (Sampaio et al., 2006a). This reflects that starters have better defensive performances in defensive rebounds associated with better jumping ability (Sampaio et al., 2006a). On the other hand, the lower fouls committed also reflect better defensive play maintaining 5 or 6 steps from one to offensive players, and this allow to successful defensive helping and reduces fouls committed (Trninic et al., 2002). One last factor seems to have influenced these results which is that nonstarters players are less worried with the 6-fouls limit that exists in the WNBA (rule 12.B. Personal foul), probably because they play less time. Thus, they could play focusing their attention on high defense pressure based on irregular hand use and physical contacts, because they are not affected by their exclusion (Sampaio et al., 2006a). Comparing the starters’ and nonstarters’ performance when they won or lost (Table 1), the descriptive results reflect that the best teams lost their games because starters’ and nonstarters’ performance was worse. In worst teams, starters and nonstarters were also discriminated by steals in won games with better values for starter players. This fact reinforces the idea of better defensive preparation in comparison with nonstarters. Trninic et al, (2002) argued that steals are a result of aggressive defensive play that decreases the offensive player’s abilities allowing to recover the ball. Thus, it is not surprising that starters are more confident and task conscious with their own performance (Gruber and Gray, 1982) and their defensive readiness is better than nonstarters resulting in an increasing number of steals. Conversely, in lost games starters were also discriminated from nonstarters by their higher number of turnovers. This result could be explained with the poor controlled style of play that starters showed in lost games, because it would increase risks in resolving game attacks and may generate more turnovers after bad passes or poor dribbling (Trninic et al., 2002). Comparing the starters’ and nonstarters’ performance when they won or lost (Table 1), the descriptive results reflect that worst teams lost their games because starters’ performance was worse. These results are different from those obtained in best teams, and reflect that best teams need better starters and nonstarters’ performances to win games, whereas in worst teams the starters’ performance should be better to win games. These facts enhance the importance of team cohesion between both groups, according to Bergeles and Hatziharistos, 2003, when starters are attracted to each other to the same competitive goal, the team’s performance is enhanced and leads to higher scoring. The prediction of group membership was similar for starters and nonstarters in all the analyses. These values were moderate, with results between 66% and 61%. These percentages reflect the ability of the discriminant function to correctly classify the players in their respective statuses (starter or nonstarter). Thus, it seems that both players’ role is less clear and that on some occasions they could be classified as starters. This fact, as Oliver, 2004 suggested, could be associated with the WNBA young life, where players showed similar performances, and only a few group of mature players have better performances than the other players. This individual dominance would configure in the league different game styles and strategies based on best players contribution. The results found in the present study indicate that men’s and women’s teams have different playing styles. These differences may reflect that women’s teams play with slower game pace (Oliver, 2004), probably women’s teams attack more frequently versus zone defenses, which require more emphasis on team and less on individual defense (Gómez et al., 2008). Thus, women’s teams need more passes and structured offenses to break the opposite defense as pointed out by the importance of offensive actions (assists and 2-point field-goals). On the other hand, men’s teams have higher game pace (Oliver, 2004; Ortega et al., 2007). This suggest better physical parameters that allow to defend with higher intensity, more physical contact and with a game based on defensive rebounds to initiate their ball possessions, and fouls trying to stop the opposite offence. |