In this study we found out that the participants reduced the time spent on developing the gate start in BMX after TREAT (1.264 ± 0.045 ms in PRE, 1.047 ± 0.019 ms in POS, and 1.041 ± 0.021 ms in RET). So, the use of audiovisual FB and cognitive training of the skill can result in a significant improvement in the execution of the gate start in BMX reducing the time to develop the task. The reduction of about 200 ms in this skill means a much better start in competitions. The gain of 200 ms in the practice means that a rider can put the elbow in front of the opponents’ that are near him. In fact, the participants of this study improved their results in the following international competitions mainly due to the improvement in their start position, and this intervention was seen by them as a successful specific training. Other studies have pointed out the improvement when performing a task, although data is not comparable. In example, non specific actions as timing a task (Chiviakowsky and Wulf, 2005), throwing an object at a target (Chiviakowsky and Wulf, 2007), or sport actions as badminton skills (Tzetzis et al., 2008) or swimming while receiving different kinds of FB (Pérez et al., 2009). Totsika and Wulf, 2003 studied the influence of external and internal foci of attention learning to ride a Pedalo®. The Pedalo® involves coordinating the entire body and, primarily, keeping balance while subjects have to pedal in two platforms in a single device in a position similar to walking. A stopwatch was used to measure movement time and the time to ride a given distance of 7 m. Twenty two subjects with no prior experience were assigned to external (n = 11) or internal (n = 11) focus of attention to learn the skill. The time needed to execute the task decreased across practice trials. Always the external focus of attention group got the best results. As the task was new for the participants, the reduction in time was very high in this non-specific sport skill, from 28 seconds to 15 seconds after 10 practices and to 12 seconds after 20 practices. So a reduction of 13 and 3 seconds were showed after 10 and 20 practices respectively. Although the reduction in time at the beginning was spectacular, this reduction was much lower across practices, being similar in the last practices. In our study the task was very well know and widely practiced in training and competitions by the participants, and instead 7 m we measured 4.5 m. Although the performance was stable at the beginning in our study, after the TREAT a reduction of 200 ms was reached. This reduction is not comparable with the one reported by Totsika and Wulf, 2003 as participants in their study were university students (elite athletes in our research) and they were learning a new skill. Also, at the latest repetitions their performances were almost regularized and stable. Finally 200 ms can be an insignificant reduction of time for some tasks, subjects or recreational contexts, while for our sample this reduction of time means giving the possibility for success in elite competitions. The results obtained in this study concur with the propositions of Wulf et al. (2002, 2005), who argue that induction to a focus of external attention can produce motor learning in participants. By focusing the subject’s attention on the effect of the movement greater use of the subject’s control processes can be encouraged. Even authors such as McNevin et al., 2003 argue that a greater distance to the external focus of attention helps to improve learning as it develops discrimination between the effect of the movement and the corporal movements that produce it, so that focusing on more distant effects result in enhanced learning by promoting the utilization of more natural control mechanisms. Hence in the present study the cut-off point of the photoelectric cell was set at 4.5 m from the point where the gate begins to fall, since these initial metres, being part of the effect of the start itself, can contribute in the sense mentioned earlier. The newer theory of administration of FB differs from the single conception that the trainer must provide information about postural imbalances that the athlete has then to interiorise (FB centred on the internal aspect), to make the corrections automatically. So, the type of FB centred on the execution and not on its result can be useful in the early stages of learning motor patterns, but has shown itself to be much less efficient both in novices and experts when improving a specific sporting skill. Furthermore, it is argued that the administration of FB centred on the result produces improvements that are maintained equally as well as those acquired by other procedures. Moreover, as shown later in the qualitative analysis of the results, the participants show that they believe they have improved in this sense. This conception goes against the idea that asserts that the manipulations of FB are more effective when they permit the subject to be aware of his/her corporal movements, as stated by Salmoni et al., 1984, Schmidt, 1991 or Schmidt and Lee (1999). On the other hand, it has been argued that the systematic use of FB on execution after every repetition can even be harmful (Konttinen et al., 2004), suggesting that administration at regular intervals of various executions can be more desirable (Wulf et al., 2002). In a recent study, Ishikura, 2008 examined the effects of reduced relative frequency of knowledge of results on learning to putt in golf. Participants putted a golf ball assigned in one of two groups (100% of knowledge of results or 33% of Knowledge of results). After the treatment the 100% group showed in the tests a larger Constant Error than the of 33% group. These results also supported that reduced relative frequency of knowledge of results was effective in learning the accuracy of the golf putt. In our case, FB on the result or performance obtained could be administered to the participants after each execution, although the subject himself decided so after each execution -being reinforced by the observer if appropriate-. In this way the participants played a responsible and active part in their learning, following the theory according to which the participants have to self-regulate their FB and make a cognitive effort in the process (Chiviakovsky and Wulf, 2007; Sherwood and Lee, 2003). The value for the asked FB in our study was 33 ± 2%, almost the same as successfully administered by Ishikura et al. (2008) of 33%. The difference is that in our study the participants asked for the FB while in Ishikura’s et al. study this value was previously decided and given by the researchers. By following an insistent pattern of FB, participants may become dependent on it, acting against the intrinsic processes of post-response FB (Wulf et al., 2002). On the other hand, self-controlled FB seems to be more effective when the learner can make a decision about receiving FB after the executions, and this can benefit learning as they can decide about FB based on the performance of a specific trial; and this was taken into account in our intervention. Also, FB after good trials was administered based in the evidences that support that learners prefer to receive FB after they believe they had a “good ”rather than a “poor ”trial (Chiviakowsky and Wulf, 2007). This was made by the observer based on the evidence of both the time spent in the execution as well as the adequation to the technical indices. Chiviakovsky and Wulf (2007) found that learning was facilitated if FB was provided after good rather than poor trials, suggesting the evidence for a motivational function of FB. Mouratidis et al., 2008 also found that motivational FB can sustain the motivational model inducing higher levels of vitality and greater intentions to participate, through the mediation of autonomous motivation. In our study the FB after good trials was provided when appropriate in 50 ± 4% of the occasions, being this % a cording to the previously and successfully administered by Chiviakowsky and Wulf, 2007 of 50% of the trials. This % was controlled by the observer trying to be as close as possible to this value. Apart of the reduction of the time spent for the task that means an evident improvement in performance, in our study a reduction in the variability of the performances was shown in terms of SD, suggesting that the participants were assimilating new motor adaptations or their performance could be more stable and consistent. The SD varied from 0.045 in PRE, 0.018 in POS, and 0.019 in RET, so taking into account that the 10 best trials of 20 were studied, this can be seen as a more stable performance of the subjects. Complementary to the practical sessions, the addition of the video session was in agreement with the idea supported by Ford et al., 2007 that concluded that performers at high levels of skill use the visual consequences of the action to plan and execute an action. This supports the idea that it is important to athletes to have a clear external view of their performance. In fact, Holmes and Calmels, 2008 suggest that imagery and observation are multicomponential, involving individual difference characteristics that modify the processes, and proposing observation-based approaches to offer more valid and effective techniques in sport psychology and motor control. In this sense, Bennet et al., 1999 argue that the importance athletes place on practice agrees with the statement made by that the variation of visual information can foster exploration during practice and makes an important teaching contribution to motor learning in sport. Here we must point out that this was the first time the participants had seen themselves on the video doing this action specifically. On the basis of the data obtained watching the video and subsequently analysing it, the participants adjusted those aspects that did not achieve consistently at the beginning and they were then able to apply them (as shown in the efficiency indices set out in Table 1). Audiovisual FB in the first instance resulted in the participants themselves making certain changes in their performances which were rapidly assimilated in a single session (although the time spent and the SD was higher than in the rest of the sessions being theses results the average for the 10 best times of 20 repetitions) while at the same time FB was administered to the participants on the result achieved. Some technical adjustments were observed related to better times in carrying out the skill. In addition to these improvements, they showed consistent improvement - times spent in the skill- two weeks later without practising the skill systematically. |