Performance analysis in ball team sports such as basketball is a fundamental tool for coaches, allowing them to have valid and reliable information concerning their team and opponents. Generally, coaches and researchers use this information to identify the most valuable players and the importance of certain specific roles (Sampaio et al., 2006a), to assess the impact of rule changes (Gómez et al., 2006), to investigate the home advantage (Carron et al., 2005; Pollard, 2008) or to evaluate the participation in the game by starting and reserve players, with the goal of determining how each player contributes to team performance (Sampaio et al., 2006b). Available research has identified differences between winning and losing teams in two- point field goals and defensive rebounds (Gómez et al., 2008; Ibáñez et al., 2003; Ittenbach and Esters, 1995; Karipidis et al., 2001). However, it seems clear that the discriminant game-related statistics change according to the games specific context, i.e., a regular season game outcome depends upon the performance in different variables than a play-off game (Sampaio and Janeira, 2003). Therefore, other game-related statistics may emerge as discriminant in other specific contexts, such as free-throws (Christoforidis et al., 2000), three-point field goal attempts and assists (Gómez et al., 2006) in competitions such as the world championships. Research also attempted to relate the championships final classification with game ball possessions, offensive and defensive ratings. After analyzing five world championships (under-18, senior, men’s, and women’s), Ibáñez et al., 2003 concluded that the best ranked teams had higher offensive coefficients and fewer ball possessions. The players and the teams’ performances can decrease after several consecutive games. For example, Montgomery et al., 2008 identified decreases in several physical capacities (speed, agility, and jump power) during 3-day tournaments. This may be due to accumulated fatigue from successive games, which affects decision-making and skill execution (Gabbett, 2008; Lyons et al., 2006; Royal et al., 2006). This way, Balciunas et al., 2006 highlighted the importance of using adequate conditioning and recovery training programs. In fact, monitoring fatigue is important, particularly in younger players who always want to perform optimally in all the stages of the game, which may have the consequence of an excessive physical exertion after various consecutive games (Griffin and Unnithan, 1999). Research has also analyzed the impact of high and moderate intensity total body fatigue in the precision of the pass, comparing expert and novice basketball players (Lyons et al., 2006). It was found that following high intensity fatigue the pass performance decreased in both groups. The results indicated that experts are better than novice when they are moderately or extremely fatigued. Furthermore, Royal et al., 2006 found that fatigue does not always affect athletes negatively because decision-making can improve when under high effort conditions. Therefore, it seems very clear that these fatigue effects are present in basketball games, particularly in tournaments with consecutive games. Concerning the topic of match analysis, it seems plausible that the game-related statistics that determine winners and losers in the first game of these 3-day tournaments may be different from those of the last game. That is, fatigue may induce different consequences in teams’ behaviour during basketball games. Thus, the aim of the present study was to identify the game- related statistics that better discriminated basketball winning and losing teams in each of the consecutive games played. |