Research article - (2009)08, 648 - 656 |
Improved Motor-Timing: Effects of Synchronized Metro-Nome Training on Golf Shot Accuracy |
Marius Sommer, Louise Rönnqvist |
Key words: Golf accuracy, motor timing, golf shot variability, metronome training |
Key Points |
|
|
|
The preset inclusion criteria for this study were healthy male golfers between 20-40 years of age, and with a golf handicap (hcp) between 0-20. For all included golfers, the start off of this study (in January) was approximately 3 months after the end of the local golf season. |
Participants |
A total of twenty-six experienced male golfers participated in this study. Their age and handicap (hcp) ranged between 20 and 37.1 years (mean 27.7), and 4.4 to 19.8 hcp (mean 12.6) respectively. After completing the pre-test, the golfers were randomly assigned to either an SMT or a Control group. The two groups did not differ significantly on any background or golf pre-test variables except for years of golf experience (t (24) = -2.877, p < 0.05). (See |
Apparatus |
Pre- and post-test golf precision measures were established in a P3ProSwing Golf Simulator located in a 5 m × 5 m × 3.5 m golf lab at Umeå University ( The ball is shot from a 22.9 cm x 35.6 cm sensing platform with 1.5 cm high artificial grass on top. The platform contains 65 optical sensors that capture information about the speed and direction of the club head at ball impact. The simulator estimates the distance and direction for each shot. According to the manufacturer (P3ProSwing, Sports Vision Technologies, California, USA), the simulator accurately monitors ball flight with 99% precision. Before this study, a number of golf shots performed in the P3ProSwing golf simulator were simultaneously measured by an Optoelectronic registration system (ProReflex, Qualisys Inc., Gothenburg, Sweden) by means of the club head velocity and angle at ball impact and compared with the P3ProSwing data. Outcomes from five different clubs (9-Iron, 4-Iron, Pitching Wedge, Driver and Putter) and in total 30 golf shots were analysed and compared. There was a high significant correlation between the two outcomes measures (overall r = 0.97). The mean velocity differences (km/h) between the two measurements were small, although consistently somewhat slower (both over repeated trials and clubs) for P3ProSwing (mean vel diff = -4.4, -4.2, -3.9, -7.4 and -0.4 km/h, respectively) in comparison to ProReflex outcomes. Similar correlations (overall r = 0.82) and differences were found for the club angle at ball impact (mean diff = 0.3, 0.23, 1.39, 0.89, 1.45 degree) for respective clubs between the two systems. Thus, we considered the outcome measures from the P3Pro simulator to be both valid and consistent. For each golf shot, accuracy was measured using the distance (in meters) between the golf ball’s final resting place and the pin (Absolute Error). Accuracy was also measured in terms of direction and distance accuracy as well as performance variability ( The Interactive Metronome (IM) ® system assessed all participants’ (SMT and Control group) timing and rhythmic skills at pre- and post-test and as training intervention for the SMT group. The IM is a computer program for Windows based on the traditional music metronome that attempts to improve and maintain timing and rhythmicity. It is set up with standard stereo headphones and a set of contact-sensing triggers, including a hand glove and a flat plastic footpad. The participants are required to perform uni- and bilateral, rhythmic hand/arm and leg/foot movements in conjunction with a computer-generated reference beat, heard through headphones ( The IM system generates scores on three dependent measures; namely the mean millisecond discrepancy between the participant’s responses and the reference beat (timing skills), the variability average that is a measure of how close each hit is timed to the previous hit (rhythmic skills), and finally the highest number of times in-a-row that the participant is able to stay within ±15 ms of the reference beat (reflecting degree of stability in performance). A high timing score indicates a larger millisecond discrepancy between the metronome beat and the participant’s movements, a score that indicates less accurate timing. Thus, lower timing scores signify better timing. |
Procedures |
First, at the pre-test occasion the participants received an explanation of the experiment protocol and provided informed consent before testing, thus, in accordance with the ethical standards specified in the Helsinki Declaration. They received 500 SEK (70 USD) for taking part in the study. Additionally, to increase the ecological validity, they were competing for a 1000 SEK (140 USD) bonus prize, information every participant received at the first pre-test occasion. All participants used their own clubs. At the time of the golf pre-test, the participants began by setting the distance from the ball (fairway) to the pin. It was emphasized that they should choose a distance from the pin that was, with some margin, within the reach of their shot with each club (4-Iron, 7-Iron and Pitching Wedge, respectively). They were informed that the same distances, with the same clubs, and under the same conditions would apply for the post-test. Before the pre-test measurement started, they could take up to five practice shots with each club to familiarize themselves to the new surface and the artificial environment. At the start of the measurement, the participants were instructed to aim for the pin and to proceed at their own pace. All golfers performed 20 test shots with each club (60 in total) in a counterbalanced randomized block design. The same procedure was used during post-test. The purpose of the IM pre- and post-test test was to assess the participant’s timing and rhythmic skills. The test is a standardized assessment developed by the instrument manufacturer, consisting of 14 different tasks, involving uni- and bi-manual hand and feet actions (Interactive Metronome, |
Intervention |
The SMT group received 12 training sessions of IM training, distributed on three 45-50 min sessions a week over a 4-week period after the post-test. The IM training was accomplished individually, and a certified IM provider was present for all sessions, monitoring the participants’ activities, modelling proper actions and correcting any technical problems. During training sessions, the IM system instantaneously transposes the timing information into discriminative, temporally based guide sounds presented in the participant’s headphones, continually indicating whether the participant was on target, early, or late. Guide sounds were not present during pre- and post-test. An early contact (i.e., a contact that precedes the beat) generates a low pitch tone in the user’s left ear. A late contact (i.e., a contact that follows the beat) generates a higher pitch tone in the right ear. A contact that matches the beat within ± 15 ms generates a higher pitched tone in the centre of the headphones and is simultaneously perceived in both ears. These instantaneous guide sounds enable the participant to correct deliberately their timing errors as they occur (for further details of the IM devise, see Libkuman et al., Due to the off-golf-season, the participants in the Control group were allowed to maintain some aspect of golf activity by performing golf swings in a commercial swing training device (Explanar Trainer®). These golfers performed in total eight 20 minute sessions with the Explanar Trainer ® distributed on two sessions a week, during a four week period between pre- and post-test. The purpose of this training was twofold; first to keep the golfers motivated to participate in the study, second; to be able to control the amount of golf-activity amongst the participants in the control group. All golfers, independent of group belonging, agreed not to take part of any other golf activity during the period between the pre- and post-test. |
Data and statistical analysis |
From the pre- and post-test outcome data of timing and rhythmic skills (IM tasks) for each golfer, we analyzed the task average (deviation from reference beat) and the variability average, which is the measure of how close each hit is timed to the previous hit (reflecting the degree of rhythmic skill). Additionally, the highest number of times in-a-row (IARs) that the golfer was able to stay within ± 15 ms of the reference beat, and the attention over time (AOT) scores were analyzed to map any possible changes in attention skills. To further investigate possible pre- to post-test improvements from the IM tasks, statistical differences were analysed by performing a mixed ANOVA with group (Control, SMT) as between-subject factors and test (pre-, post-test) as within-subject factors, using repeated measurement on dependent measures. For analysing possible differences between groups, tests, and possible interactions regarding the pre- and post-test outcome data from the golf shots (made in the golf simulator), all accuracy records (Absolute Error, Distance and Direction Error, and Variability in performance) were further analysed by separate mixed ANOVAs with use of repeated measures. Additionally, as the participants set the distance from the ball (fairway) to the pin individually, we divided the golfers into two sub-groups; low (< 10.9) and high (> 10.9) hcp golfers (resulting in 6 high hcp- and 7 low hcp golfers in respective group) to analyse possible effects of golf handicap on self selected distance between ball and pin. Scheffe’s post hoc test was used on all significant effects, and the pre set alpha level was 0.05. |
|
|
For Task Average, a 2 (group: SMT and Control) × 2 (test: pre- and post-test) ANOVA revealed no main effect for group; F (1, 24) = 3.1, p = 0.09. However, a significant effect of test; F (1, 24) = 37.2, p < 0.0001, as well as a significant interaction between group and test; F (1, 24) = 25.3, p < 0.0001, was found. The post-hoc comparisons showed that only the SMT group differed significantly (p < 0.001) between pre-and post-test scores ( For Task Variability, the ANOVA revealed no main effect for group; F (1, 24) = 3.06, p = 0.09, however, a significant effect of test; F (1, 24) = 91.39, p < 0.0001, as well as a significant interaction between group and test; F (1, 24) = 29.85, p < 0.0001, was found. In agreement with the findings from Task Average, the post-hoc comparisons showed that only the SMT group differed significantly (p < 0.01) between pre-and post-test scores ( For AOT, the ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of group; F (1, 24) = 6.54, p < 0.05, as well as of test; F (1, 24) = 13.1, p = 0.01. Additionally, a significant interaction between group and test; F (1, 24) = 35.60, p < 0.001, was found. The post-hoc comparisons showed that the SMT group significantly differed (p < 0.001) between pre-and post-test scores for AOT; in addition, a significant difference (p < 0.05) between the SMT and the Control group for the post-test scores was found ( For IAR, the ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of test; F (1, 24) = 34.14, p < 0.001, and group; F (1, 24) = 9.78, p < 0.001. Furthermore, a significant interaction between group and test; F (1, 24) = 19.81, p < 0.001, was found. The post-hoc comparisons revealed that the SMT group significantly differed (p < 0.001) between pre- and post-test scores for number of IARs, in addition, a significant difference (p < 0.01) between the SMT and the Control group for the post-test scores was found ( |
Golf accuracy |
As each participant set their own distance from the ball (fairway) to the pin, based on their individual judgement on what was the reach of the shot with each club (4-Iron, 7- Iron and Pitching Wedge, respectively), a 2 (group: SMT and Control) x 2 (Handicap: High and Low) x 3 (Clubs) MANOVA, with group and handicap as between factors and clubs as a within factor and with repeated measures over the last factor, was initially conducted. Thus, to investigate the existence of possible group differences in relation to handicap level and the distance chosen between fairway and the pin. No main effect of group F (1, 22) = 0.52, p = 0.48, or handicap; F (1, 22) = 2.28, p = 0.15, was found. However, as expected a significant main effect of clubs; F (2, 44) = 615.5, p = < 0.001, was evident. Independently of group and handicap, the mean distance chosen for respective clubs was 175 m for the 4-Iron; 152 m for the 7-Iron; and 115 m for the Pitching Wedge. No significant Group x Handicap (p = 0.39); Group x Clubs (p = 0.48), or Group x Handicap x Clubs (p = 0.35), interactions were found. A 2 (group: SMT and Control) × 2 (test: pre- and post-test) x 3 (club: 4-Iron, 7-Iron and Pitching Wedge) mixed-design ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of group; F (1, 24) = 0.99, p = 0.33. However, a main effect of test; F (1, 24) = 4.35, p < 0.05, as well as a main effect of clubs; F (2, 23) = 34.68, p < 0.0001, was found. The post-hoc test revealed that the absolute error by the Pitching Wedge (9.7 m) was found to be significantly shorter (p < 0.01) than the absolute error by the 7-Iron (13.2 m) and the 4-Iron (14.5 m), respectively. Furthermore, a significant Group × Test interaction was found; F (1, 24) = 12.03, p < 0.01 ( The post-hoc comparisons showed a significant (p < 0.01) difference between the pre- to post-test for the SMT group by means of an overall increasing accuracy (decreasing distance to the pin) at the post-test in comparison to the pre-rest. Such improvement was not found for the Control group. In addition, the improvement showed by the SMT group by means of a decreasing distance to the pin was found to be consistent over all clubs ( As the number of years of experience differed between the two groups, a mixed-design ANCOVA was further conducted (on AE) using experience (number of years) as a covariate. The ANCOVA revealed no main effects of group; F (1, 23) = 0.54, p = 0.47, or test; F (1, 23) = 0.056, p = 0.81. However, the Group x Test interaction was still found to be significant; F (1, 23) = 8.29, p < 0.01. Thus, this finding confirmed the result from the previous analysis. Consequently, golf experience does not explain the outcome differences between the SMT- and Control group found at the post-test. For the Direction measure, the ANOVA revealed no significant main effects of group; F (1, 24) = 1.23, p = 0.28, or test; F (1, 24) = 0.96, p = 0.34. However, a significant Group × Test interaction was evident; F (1, 24) = 6.35, p < 0.05. In agreement with the outcome from the distance, the post-hoc comparisons revealed that the direction improvement between pre-post-tests was only evident (p < 0.05) for the SMT group ( |
|
|
Timing and rhythmicity training programs have been used in a variety of rehabilitation settings with documented success. Based on that success, the concept of timing training has also gained popularity in applied fitness settings to enhance sport performance. However, few studies have looked at the efficacy of timing training and its effect on sport performance. The present study was designed to determine the efficacy of Synchronized Metronome Training (SMT) on motor timing and to determine its possible effect of improved motor timing on golf shot accuracy using a pre-test/post-test design in groups of experienced male golfers. As with Libkuman et al., One explanation in line with Libkuman et al., For example, Meegan et al., An alternative explanation is that SMT does improve the golfer’s ability to concentrate and stay focused. There are two results pointing towards changes in the participant’s attention and focus. First, the SMT significantly increased the mean number of IARs (hits in a row within ± 15ms of the reference beat) the participants could achieve. We interpret this as an improvement of the participant’s ability to attend to the task at hand. Secondly, the significant decrease in the deviation from the reference beat during the AOT test (10 consecutive minutes of matching the reference beat) strengthens the notion that SMT improves the participant’s ability to attend selectively to a stimulus without interruption by internal thoughts or external distractions for extended periods. Similarly, Diamond, |
Limitations and future research |
It is not clear from the findings from the SMT whether number of repetitions, length of training sessions, alternative timing exercises, and a different reference beat tempo (longer/shorter) might affect the results differently. Furthermore, there is also a need for further investigations of what type of sensory feedback (by means of the instantaneous provided feedback - auditory and/or visual - that enables the participant to deliberately correct their timing errors as they occur) may optimize and/or affect the outcome of IM training. In addition, the possible long-term effects of SMT are unknown and in need of evaluation. Thus, future research will be necessary to further delineate the phenomenon and to develop a theory that can explain how the property of timing influences the complex motor activity in golf performance. Thus, we plan subsequent investigations of the kinematics properties and dynamics of the golf swing performance and how timing training by means of SMT may affect the kinematics. However, such investigation was beyond the scope of the present study. |
|
|
The present study showed a significant effect of SMT by means of improvements in motor timing and synchronizations. Additionally, significantly improved scores on selected golf shot accuracy variables and with clearly decreased variability after just 4-weeks of training were evident. As the present study shows, sensory motor control and golf shot accuracy outcomes were positively affected by SMT. This suggests that enhanced and optimal golf shot accuracy does require precise, timed, and synchronized sensory motor control. The finding that improvements of golf shot accuracy are positively affected by improved motor timing and that such improvement occurs after just a 4-weeks intervention without any sport specific training has interesting implications for other sports as well. For example, other athletes could benefit from such a complementary training method. Additionally, the SMT method may also be useful during periods of limited and/or impaired sensory motor functions. |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
This study was supported by a Swedish grant from “Centrum för Idrottsforskning ”(CIF) / Centre for Sport Science. The authors would like to thank Anna-Maria Johansson for assisting with the IM intervention program. Sincere appreciation is also expressed to the participating golfers for their commitment to the study. The present experiment complies with the current laws of Sweden. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|