Match analysis is the objective recording and examination of behavioural events that occur during competition (Carling et al., 2005). The main aim of match analysis is to identify strengths of one’s own team, which can then be further developed, and its weaknesses, which suggest areas for improvement. Similarly, a coach analysing the performance of an opposition side will use the data to identify ways to counter that team’s strengths and exploit its weaknesses (Carling et al., 2009). Performance indicators are defined as the selection and combination of variables that define some aspect of performance and help achieve athletic success (Hughes and Bartlett, 2002). These indicators constitute an ideal profile that should be present in the athletic activity to achieve success and can be used as a way to predict the future behaviour of sporting activity (O´Donoghue, 2005). Empirical research investigating match analysis in soccer has generally been focused upon goal scoring and patterns of build-up play leading to shots (Ensum et al., 2002; Grant et al., 1999; Hook and Hughes, 2001; Hughes et al., 1988; Hughes and Churchill, 2005; Hughes and Franks, 2005; Jones et al., 2004; Konstadinidou and Tsigilis, 2005; Scoulding et al., 2004; Stanhope, 2001; Yamanaka et al., 1993). Some of these studies relate these aspects to the result of the game (winning or losing). However, playing patterns within previous studies have shown relatively contradictory findings. For example Hughes, Robertson and Nicholson (1988) found that teams who reached the semi finals of the 1986 World Cup tended to occupy the centre of the pitch more often, whereas those that failed to progress beyond the group stages utilised the wings. In addition, when the ball was regained, attempts at goal were also significantly greater for the successful team. However, whilst this study provided some evidence of different patterns of play between teams deemed successful or unsuccessful, the findings may be less applicable to modern football due to the time period in which it was conducted. However a similar investigation was completed by Low et al., 2002 on 40 matches within the 2002 soccer World Cup which produced similar results to those of Hughes et al., 1988 although no statistics were utilised to compare the differences between the teams. Further investigations have been completed on playing patterns within World Cups, but have tended to focus on a single team. For example Griffiths, 1999 selected matches involving France, who were at this time considered the best international team in the World. It was reported that France was able to create significantly more shots while also having the ability to retain possession for long periods. Interestingly France also created significantly more crosses than their opponents, which suggests that in modern soccer successful teams may utilise wing attacks more often than reported in earlier research (e.g. Hughes et al., 1988). Scoulding et al., 2004 suggest that in terms of passes in different areas of the pitch very little difference existed between the most successful and an unsuccessful team during the 2002 World Cup. Hughes and Franks, 2005 compared the performance of successful and unsuccessful teams in 1990 World Cup. They found differences between the two in converting possession into shots on goal, with the successful teams having the better ratios. However, Hughes and Churchill, 2005 compared the pattern of play of successful and unsuccessful teams leading to shots and goals during the Copa America Tournament of 2001. They found that there were no significant differences between the successful and unsuccessful team’s patterns of play leading to shots. Grant et al., 1999 analysed the 1998 World Cup and concluded that successful teams (reached the semi-finals) were able to penetrate the defence by passing, running or dribbling the ball in a forward direction for longer sequences of play than unsuccessful teams (failed to pass the initial group stage). Employing similar methods Hook and Hughes, 2001 found that successful teams utilised longer possessions than unsuccessful teams in Euro 2000, although no significant differences were found in the number of passes used in attacks leading to a goal. These authors suggested that keeping the ball for longer durations was indicative of success. However, in a similar study Stanhope, 2001 found that time in possession of the ball was not indicative of success in the 1994 World Cup. Jones, et al. (2004) showed that successful teams in the English Premier league typically had longer possessions than unsuccessful teams irrespective of the match status (evolving score). Other studies have tried to provide a ‘formula’ of winning by reporting statistics of successful teams on the assumption that mimicking these figures would create a “winning formula”. For example, Horn et al., 2002, identified a specific part of the pitch, the central area just outside the penalty area. It was suggested that 86% of passes into this area would subsequently enter the penalty area and thus likely to provide shooting opportunities. In a similar vein, Taylor and Williams, 2002 cited the importance of retained possession for the winners of the 2002 World Cup finals and suggested that possession gained in the defensive area resulted in more attempts on goal than for the other teams. This finding is similar to the ideas suggested by Pollard, 2002 who discussed the ability to win matches with regard to the number of actions performed and were deemed successful, which he called ‘yield’. It was suggested that unsuccessful teams would display a lower yield although categorical conclusions like this are impossible to substantiate in a sport like soccer where a late goal can completely alter the result of a match. The analysis of game statistics, with regards to individual and collective skills, is one of the tools that can be utilized to describe and monitor behaviour in competition (Ortega et al., 2009). In spite of the limitations that can arise from the different variables used in these studies (Hughes et al., 2002), this type of data is useful to have greater knowledge of the game. Although such studies examined indicators of success in soccer, some limitations and/or methodological problems in the study of these aspects can be observed. Many of these studies failed to demonstrate the reliability of the data gathering system used (Hughes et al., 2001). Indeed, Hughes and Franks, 1997 suggest that all computerised notation system should be tested for intra-observer reliability (repeatability). Also, selecting matches from a one-off tournament means that the selected teams (successful and unsuccessful) are not balanced in terms of the strength of opposition and number of matches played. Moreover, the findings should be approached with caution as the results have been gained through analysis of limited numbers of teams and as such may not be applicable to all teams. Finally, these studies are based on small sample sizes and, largely, an univariate analysis of the observed variable is done. These factors are likely to influence a team’s performance and may therefore contribute to the differences found in existing studies. Based on the limitations of the extant research, the purpose on the present study was to identify which game-related statistics allow discriminating winning, drawing and losing teams in the Spanish Professional League. |