Bilateral able-bodied gait asymmetries are well documented (Allard et al., 1996; Arsenault et al., 1986; Herzog et al., 1989; Maupas et al., 1999; Ounpuu and Winter, 1989; Sadeghi et al., 1997; 2000; Seeley et al., 2008), yet their causes have not been fully explained. LLI may be one cause of gait asymmetries (Du Chatinier and Rozendal, 1970; Gurney et al., 2001; Kaufman et al., 1996; Perttunen et al., 2004; Subotnick, 1981; White et al., 2004), and the purpose of this study was to investigate the nature of the relationship between LLI and able-bodied gait asymmetries. This was accomplished by accurately quantifying LLI and gait asymmetries within a relatively large sample of subjects. Our results showed that LLI was not significantly related to the degree of bilateral symmetry for joint angles during gait (Table 2). Our results do, however, support the idea that LLI influences gait symmetry for joint moment and power at the hip, knee, and ankle joints (Table 2). Each of the correlations for moments and powers were negative (r values ranged from -0.36 to -0.51), and nearly all of them met statistical significance (Table 2). The two p values that exceeded 0.05 (hip moment: 0. 053, and hip power: 0.072) did so only by small margins. Overall, these moderate negative correlations indicate that in able-bodied walking, subjects with larger LLI tend to exhibit a greater degree of asymmetry in joint kinetics. It should be emphasized, however, that the highest r value was -0.51, meaning that no more than 26% of the observed gait asymmetries can be explained by LLI. Other morphological asymmetries (e.g., segment mass or moment of inertia) or neuromuscular factors probably also contributed to the observed gait asymmetries. Some of the other possible causes of gait asymmetries could actually be long-term adaptations to the structural differences in leg length. The results from the comparisons between subjects with relatively small and relatively large LLI reinforced the idea that LLI influences joint moment and power symmetry during gait. Subjects with relatively large LLI showed significantly less symmetrical gait for knee and ankle moment and power than subjects with relatively small LLI (Table 3). In combination, the moderate negative correlations described in the previous paragraph, and the between-group (large vs. small LLI) differences described here, indicate that bilateral symmetry for joint moments and powers is related to LLI. In contrast, our results provided little evidence that joint angle asymmetries are strongly influenced by LLI. Particularly at the hip and knee, the joint angles were so bilaterally symmetrical that there was little variance to be explained by LLI, or any other potential variable. This may reflect a tendency for humans to maintain stable and consistent kinematics, by varying joint kinetics. This phenomenon, which has been referred to as kinematic invariance, has been reported for normal (Borghese et al., 1996; Winter, 1983) and amputee gait (Selles et al., 2004). This idea of kinematic invariance fits well with our findings and suggests that humans can maintain a high degree of kinematic symmetry during walking, despite varying degrees of LLI. While the statistical analyses indicated moderate, negative relationships between LLI and most of the moment and power variables, some aspects of these findings warrant further comment. First, it should be noted that these negative relationships were due in part to results from a relatively small number of subjects who exhibited the most substantial asymmetries for joint angle, moment, and power (Table 2 and Figure 3). It would be tempting to consider some of these subjects as outliers. However, we did not exclude these subjects from the statistical analyses, as their presentation was in fact consistent with our hypotheses (i. e., larger LLI is associate with greater asymmetry). In contrast, none of the subjects with relatively low (< 1.0 cm) LLI exhibited large asymmetries for any variable. However, there tended to be more variability in the symmetry coefficient values for the moment and power variables than for the angle variables (Figure 3), even in the low LLI group. For subjects with relatively large LLI (≥ 1.0 cm), there was considerable variability in the symmetry coefficient values for most of the kinetic variables (Figure 3), with some subjects maintaining nearly perfect symmetry, while other exhibit substantial gait asymmetries for some variables. Thus, another way to interpret the statistical results is that subjects with no meaningful LLI (i.e., LLI < 1.0 cm) walk with highly symmetrical gait kinematics and kinetics, whereas it is difficult to predict the amount of asymmetry that a subject with a larger LLI will exhibit. The subjects with larger LLI and high gait symmetry must have made neuromuscular adaptations to compensate for the structural asymmetry. It is not clear why and how these subjects made the necessary adaptations, or why some of the other subjects with larger LLI did not. It is also worth commenting on the technique that was used to quantify the degree of gait symmetry for each subject. Our symmetry coefficient was based on the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (Arsenault et al., 1986; Pierotti et al., 1991). The Pearson coefficient has the advantage of being simple and familiar, and takes into account the whole gait cycle, not just discrete estimates. The Pearson coefficient is sensitive to differences in both timing and amplitude in the curves being compared (Derrick et al., 1994), which can be a limitation in certain applications. The present data did exhibit asymmetries in both timing and amplitude (Figure 2). Our intent was to quantify the degree of symmetry in general, rather than any specific aspects of it. Therefore, the Pearson coefficient was deemed to be an appropriate index of gait symmetry for the present study. Any attempt to more finely characterize the nature of the asymmetries would require the use of another approach. A primary finding of this study was greater asymmetry for kinetic variables, compared to joint angles. While we believe the greater asymmetry for kinetic variables to be genuine, it is possible that cumulative errors in the data processing procedures may have contributed to these findings. Joint angles are subject to error related to reflective marker position data and skin movement relative to the underlying bony tissue (Cappozzo et al., 1996). Joint moments are subject to these same errors, but also to uncertainty in body segment inertial parameters (e.g., Pearsall and Costigan, 1999) and alignment of the kinematic and force plate reference frames (e.g., McCaw and DeVita, 1995). The computation of joint powers will carry forward all of these sources of error, and further involves time differentiation of the joint angle data. Thus, it is possible that accumulated error may have contributed to the trends in asymmetries from joint angle, to moments, to powers. The present results have important clinical implications. Several of our subjects exhibited LLI that we operationally defined as relatively large (> 1 cm), and the lowest symmetry coefficients were found among these subjects. However, many clinicians, especially those still relying on inaccurate tape measures, may ignore LLIs of this magnitude and consider them to be normal. Also, LLIs described as relatively large (> 1 cm) in the present study have been deemed pathologic (Friberg, 1982; Subotnick, 1981); however, all of the present subjects were asymptomatic. This suggests that for walking, it may not be feasible to identify a simple threshold magnitude for LLI that, if surpassed, will result in pathology and/or necessitate clinical intervention (Gurney, 2002; Reid and Smith, 1984). Such a threshold would likely vary among individuals, and would depend on many factors, such as anthropometrics, age, and activity level (Gurney, 2002; Reid and Smith, 1984). On the other hand, it should be noted that our subjects were relatively young, and symptoms related to LLI (e.g., osteoarthritis or back pain) may require more time to manifest. Related to this point, a longitudinal study of the cumulative effects of LLI on gait asymmetry is warranted. Finally, our results imply that clinicians should expect a certain degree of bilateral asymmetry in kinetic variables, and should consider this asymmetry when evaluating unilateral pathology. Asymmetrical joint kinematics, however, appear to be less typical and are more likely to indicate a need for clinical intervention because they will likely be associated with extremely asymmetric patterns of kinetics and energetics. Our results for able-bodied walkers were generally consistent with previous research regarding gait symmetry and LLI for impaired subjects (Kaufman et al., 1996; Perttunen et al., 2004). Conclusions from this prior research also indicated that gait symmetry decreases with increases in LLI. The present data also agree with White et al., 2004 who showed various components of the ground reaction force become less symmetrical with increases in LLI. However, the present data differed from the results of Gurney et al., 2001 concerning the relationship between LLI and symmetry for quadriceps and plantarflexor muscle electromyographic activity, as measured with surface electromyography (Gurney et al., 2001). They reported that quadriceps and plantarflexor electromyographic activity are symmetrical in subjects with a LLI up to 3 cm (Gurney et al., 2001). This difference in results may be partially explained by the different variables considered. Electromyography data tend to be more variable than joint kinetics and kinematics, which would make asymmetry more difficult to detect. This study was the first to quantify the relationship between LLI and gait asymmetry, using radiography and inverse dynamics analysis, to better understand the relationship between LLI and able-bodied gait symmetry. The use of radiography to determine LLI is an important component of this study, because previous evaluations of the relationship used less accurate methods for quantifying LLI such as the tape measure (White et al., 2004) and wooden block (Woerman and Binder-MacLeod, 1984). When considering the implications of LLI, it is critical to use radiography (Gurney, 2002), as other methods are unreliable (McCaw and Bates, 1991) and lack the precision necessary to accurately measure LLI. |