The management of discipline is an essential element in education practice (Buluc, 2006). Given that there is more interaction in physical education than in other school subjects, the question of discipline in this subject has been one of the main pedagogical concerns of teachers and a key element for successful teaching (Mawer, 1995; Siedentop, 1991). Over the last decades, researchers have associated the issue of discipline with the interest and motivation for learning (Chen, 2001; Graham, 2008). Recently, a small body of literature has established that motivational orientation and the perceptions of motivational climate are associated with pupils’ self-reported discipline (Cervelló et al., 2004; Moreno-Murcia et al., 2008; Papaioannou, 1998b; Spray, 2002; Spray and Wang, 2001). To be precise, in line with this work, the purpose of this study was to test a motivational model of discipline, which integrates the situational and dispositional elements proposed by achievement goal theory and which helps to interpret gender differences in self-reported discipline. These results provide strong support for the proposed model and make it possible to explain gender differences as well as suggest some pedagogical implications. The results of this study revealed that dispositional goal orientation and the perception of motivational climate were related to students’ self-reported discipline/indiscipline. More specifically, the perception of an ego-involving motivational climate and an ego orientation was related positively to self-reported indiscipline and negatively to self-reported discipline, whereas the perception of a task-involving climate and a task orientation was correlated positively to self-reported discipline and negatively to self- reported indiscipline. These results were also borne out by the motivational model tested, since the more task-involving the teacher’s behavior was perceived by students, the more self-reported discipline there was. Conversely, the more ego-involving the teacher’s behavior, the more self-reported indiscipline there was. In turn, the less task orientation, the less self-reported discipline and the more self-reported indiscipline. In contrast, the less ego orientation, the less self-reported indiscipline and the more self-reported discipline there were. Results from both the correlation analysis and the SEM seem coherent with hypotheses defended by the achievement goals theory, since task-oriented persons are more likely to endorse fairness in physical and sport activities, but highly ego-oriented individuals tend to adopt unfair and illegal means to achieve their goals (Duda et al., 1991). As success is based on normative comparison, personal improvement is not sufficient to satisfy the goal of demonstrating superior ability (Xiang et al., 2003a; 2003b). Therefore, ego-involved students must pay more attention to classmates than to the task owing to the fact that they have to outperform others to perceive a sense of achievement. Ego- oriented students are motivated by external factors and, consequently, they do not perceive that it is necessary to obey the learning rules in order to achieve the proposed aims (Cervelló et al., 2004; Moreno-Murcia et al., 2008). Furthermore, students are more likely to respond with an adaptive behavior when the meaning of achievement emphasized in physical education classes is based on personal improvement and effort levels rather than on performance and ability. Teachers who foster a performance-oriented climate are more likely to emphasize control than those who do not emphasize ego orientation in their classes (Papaioannou, 1995). Similarly, teachers who emphasize normative comparison are perceived as more severe than instructors who are not interested in who is the best (Cervelló et al., 2004). In this vein, when students perceive a severe climate, they could show great difficulty in carrying out teachers’ requirements, which would have an effect on their perception of indiscipline-discipline. The ANOVA results revealed that boys had a higher level of ego-orientations, ego-involving motivational climate perceptions and self-reported indiscipline than girls. In this respect, the SEM results supported the hypotheses that boys believed they were more undisciplined than girls and this self-report was associated with their goal orientation and the different way they perceived the physical education class climate. That is, more ego motivational orientation and the perception that teachers emphasized performance in their classes probably lead to more self-reported indiscipline in boys than in girls. These findings seem coherent with the studies that have analyzed the gender variable with regards to the student’s motivation and behavior in physical education situations. As far as motivational differences are concerned, research usually indicates that boys tend to perceive an ego-involving climate and be more ego-oriented than girls (Carr and Weigand, 2001; Duda and Whitehead, 1998; Ntoumanis and Biddle, 1999; White et al., 1998; Spray and Biddle, 1997). On the other hand, several studies have demonstrated how gender determines children’s perception within physical education classes and how it affects task engagement. Specifically, research that has examined the treatment of children of different genders has demonstrated that girls receive less positive feedback and do not have the same chances to participate as boys (Macdonald, 1990; Lirgg, 1993). As studies have shown, teachers usually encourage boys more strongly than they do girls and children probably perceive this unequal treatment (Cervelló et al., 2004; Papaioannou, 1995). However, even though more attention towards boys has usually been observed as an advantage for their learning chances (Lirgg, 1993), the results of this study suggest that it could foster an ego-involving climate and produce maladjusted behaviors. In this respect, male students who perceive more attention than their female peers will probably increase their sense of perceived pressure due to an increase in the teacher’s control and a drop in the perception of their autonomy (Papaioannou, 1998a; 1998b). In addition, students who perceive that decisions are only made by the teacher are more likely not to feel responsible for applying the rules established in class (Buluc, 2006). In contrast, the feeling of self-responsibility has been more associated with task orientation and a perceived task-involved climate than with ego orientation and an ego-involved climate (Newton et al., 2006). In this vein, Morgan et al., 2005 showed that pupil-centered teaching styles resulted in more mastery and less performance focused teaching behaviors and more adaptive cognitive and effective responses than teacher-centered teaching styles. Similarly, Papaioannou, 1998a founded that the perception of an ego-involving climate was related to the perception of the teacher’s control of the children’s behaviors. As an emphasis on performance goals does not increase students’ motivation, nor is it perceived as desirable teaching behavior (Papaiannou, 1998b), teachers, therefore, should try to help students, especially boys, develop self-determination and internalize the value of discipline. The SEM results also suggest that the impact of motivational climates on the self-report of indiscipline/discipline is not only mediated by dispositional motivation, but there is also a direct effect of the climate perceived in class on levels of self-reporting discipline. So far, research, in accordance with the achievement goal theory, has sustained the idea that perceived motivational climate could modify the subject’s dispositional motivation and this change would affect his strategies and behaviors (Nicholls, 1984; 1989). Our model suggests that, although the impact of perceived motivational climates is mediated by motivational orientations, the environments created by the teacher also have a direct impact on self-reported discipline. An interesting observation in this study is that, considering the relationship of the task-involving motivational climate, both direct and mediated by task orientation, it predicted 51% of the variance in self-reported discipline, while the ego-involving climate predicted, directly or combined with ego orientation, 56% of the variance in self-reported indiscipline. In addition, teachers’ behavior seems to have a direct impact on levels of self-reported discipline, although the latter is also influenced by the changes that can occur in the pupil’s dispositional orientation. Therefore, according to the motivational model tested, the gender differences found in the ANOVA in the perception of a task-involving motivational climate are probably connected with the differences found in their self reports of discipline in two ways. Firstly, the highest perception of a task-involving climate that the boys have in comparison with the girls may explain the differences in their self-reported indiscipline. Secondly, the influence that the perception of an ego-involving motivational climate, mediated by ego orientation, also has on self-reported discipline could be an explanation for the fact that girls demonstrate more self-reported discipline, although the ANOVA did not indicate gender differences in task orientation and the perception of a task-involving motivational climate. Nevertheless, although some evidence has been found for the mediational role of the subject’s motivational disposition on certain behaviors in sport and physical education situations (Cury et al., 1996; Gano-Overway and Ewing, 2004; Sarrazin et al., 2002), this study’s main contribution is that, in accordance with the hypothesis made, motivation-al climate has a direct role to play on self-reported indiscipline/discipline. Finally, the model tested in this study has also enabled us to notice the relative impact that motivational climates have had on self-reported discipline. The results of this study indicate that direct paths from task-involving and ego-involving motivational climates to self-reported discipline/indiscipline were significantly stronger than those paths from task and ego orientation to self-reported discipline/indiscipline. A potential explanation for the presence of the climate effect is that it is probably more important than motivational orientation for situations that have not been chosen freely by the subject. Some studies have suggested that in situations where motivations are more extrinsic than leisure activities, a subject that does not enjoy it or feels compelled will probably quickly decide to drop out from the activity (Sarrazin et al., 2002). In those situations, such as in physical education classes where the activity is obligatory and the subject don’t have an opportunity to drop out from the activity, their response could lead to a higher predisposition to not follow requirements in accordance with established norms. Prior studies have suggested that for compulsory physical education settings, situational influences can have a stronger impact than goal orientations on motivation to learn (Cury et al., 1996; Spray, 2000). Our findings support these studies and suggest that the perceived climate in classes plays a more important role than the motivational orientation the pupil comes to class with when informing about their behavior. Nevertheless, it is also possible that the relative importance of these paths depends on the content and the social and cultural meanings assigned to them. Future research is needed to understand better the relative impact of motivational climates created by the teacher on the way pupils perceive their behavior in class. To sum up, these findings provide strong support for the model as applied to self-reported discipline in physical education settings. Nevertheless, some limitations should be acknowledged when interpreting the results. Firstly, measurement of the variables was self-reported data and the use of these measurements may create a variance error in the model. Furthermore, it should be recognized that the relationships between motivation and discipline are certainly affected by social and not only instructional goals. In this vein, although the achievement goal theory is a suitable framework in the study of student motivation in achievement settings, this research suggests that goal theory alone may not fully explain student motivational processes and their connections with classroom behaviors, such as indiscipline/discipline. Based on the achievement goal theory framework, indiscipline has been considered as a child’s problem to adapt. However, studies have found that certain disruptive behavior and school children’s physical aggression in early adolescence, especially by boys, usually aims to maintain or gain status with some peers (Adler and Adler, 1995; Jiménez et al., 2008; Samper et al., 2008; Zimmer-Gembeck et al., 2005). Finally, research of student motivational processes has advanced beyond the realm of psychology and, consequently, research from an integrated perspective has been recommended (Chen, 2001). Qualitative measures, including observations and interviews with pupils, could also prove helpful in uncovering potential significant reasons for student behaviors. |