Excellent performance in sport has a strong positive relationship with the accumulated number of hours of practice, and the specialization years are seen as a decisive moment to lift an athlete’s skill level, readiness and commitment (De Bruin et al., 2007; Gonçalves et al., 2009). The assumption behind the argument is that experts are always made, not born (Ericsson et al., 2007). This theory translated to the youth sport domain means that if an athlete wants to be a real high level performer, he/she needs a deliberate engagement in practice during the specialization years, spending time wisely and always focusing on tasks that challenge the current performance. The Deliberate Practice model raises an important issue to sport pedagogy because it led the sport organizations responsible for the development of young sport talents to increase the amount of hours spent in organized practice, under the supervision of specialized coaches at increasingly lower ages. It seems reasonable that if the young athletes are better selected, have better training conditions and practice and compete more time with better teammates and opponents, the chance of becoming competent adult athletes is greater. The 10.000 hours rule and the Long Term Athlete Development model (Bályi and Williams, 2009; Ericsson et al., 2007) express this perspective, proposing a training volume sequentially balanced through more or less 10 years of sport specialization. In addition, Chi, 2006 argues that the road to expertise means that efforts must be consistent focused on weaknesses improvement and produce successful outcomes (winning the competitions). Trying to adjust to the Deliberate Practice model many sport organizations in several countries around the world created specialized training centres where selected young talents practice under the supervision of experienced coaches in order to become professional athletes and integrate youth national teams. In team sports this process has been adopted by professional clubs or national sports associations, and starts usually at around 14 years of age. In most cases, a part or all the youngsters live, go to school and practice at the training campus, isolated from the families and native environments. This choice towards an elite restricted group at early ages raises the problem of talent identification and selection. The concept of readiness, associated with growth and maturation (Malina et al., 2004) underlines the risks associated with high training loads and the complexity of decision making about future performances of immature individuals. To recruit adolescents or pre-adolescents to join a demanding training program in a full commitment basis is a complex task that should be well scientifically grounded, in order to prevent errors in prognosis. Approaching this critical issue, Helsen et al., 2000, or Elferink-Gemser et al., 2007 argued that the selection and orientation of talent has been strongly dependent on biological and motor variables, although these variables are not able to fully differentiate athletes by competitive levels. It is believed that in order to engage in a demanding schedule of year-long practice, individuals have to be highly motivated. Moreover, most research pointed out that an orientation for mastery achievement is critical for overcoming challenging motor tasks (Duda, 2001; Roberts, 2001), and a competitive, ego achievement orientation has been described as a deterrent factor for enjoyment in practice and will to continue activity (Sarrazin and Guillet, 2001; Sage and Kavussanu, 2007). However, the pursuit of expertise in sport means that progress in performance must be constantly evaluated and the most efficient kind of evaluation is competition, with its wins-losses record. If practice is oriented to improve performance, it is reasonable to expect that athletes show a strong interest in competitive outcomes and see victory as an important moment of the process. Gould, Dieffenbach and Moffett (2002), in a study with Olympic champions found that they are very competitive and self-confident. Harwood et al., 2004 in a study with young performers showed that they express a high task-/ego-orientation, and argue that elite athletes cope better with competitive stress when their achievement orientations are both high. The concept of elite young athletes has not been clearly defined. McArdle and Duda, 2004 considered elite those who participate in competitions in national championships. Elferink-Gemser et al., 2004 considered as talented field hockey players those who participate in the national youth teams programs, a decision that fits with a definition of elite as a restricted group of gifted individuals. In this study we consider as elite those players selected by the national Basketball Association to live and practice in high performance centres and play for the national teams in European competitions. Hence, the decision to engage in such programs should be founded on a clear orientation towards competitive success and on a strong will to become an expert player, ready to practice at the standards of volume and intensity required by excellent performance. Although the athletes are first of all adolescents, the characteristics we expect to discriminate elite players from their peers playing at a lower level are achievement orientations and the will to become experts through deliberate practice. We argue that it would be useful for coaches and families who carry the responsibility of choice and lead the young performers to have better information about important non biological variables when making decisions that can influence all the youngsters’ life. The Work and Family Orientation Questionnaire (WOFO; Spence and Helmreich, 1983) assesses general achievement orientations, independent of task or context. The instrument contains four sub-scales: work (the desire to practice and perform well), mastery (the desire to face challenging tasks), competitiveness (the desire to be better when compared to others), and personal unconcern (lack of concern with others’ opinions). The reliability of the WOFO questionnaire has been tested in sports (Gill, 1988). According to De Bruin et al., 2007, the personal unconcern scale has little value in achievement research, and only the other three scales were used in the study. In this work we hypothesized that mastery, work and competitiveness contribute to predict the belonging to the elite or local level. Besides dispositional general achievement orientations, our purpose was to assess the situational specific circumstances that could motivate the youngsters to engage in deliberate practice in high performance centres. De Bruin et al., 2007, in a study with young chess players, argue that to stay focused on a task for long periods of time, besides achievement orientation, specific motivation for a specific sport is determinant. The same authors designed an instrument, called Deliberate Practice Motivation Questionnaire (DPMQ), to assess the individuals’ will to become an excellent performer and to improve in competition. Although in team sports there is no objective measure of the evolution of performance of a single player, the theoretical framework of deliberate practice is useful to the same purposes with basketball young elite athletes. Therefore we adapted the DPMQ to Basketball both to long time goals (“I want to be a professional Basketball player”), and to specific changing situations of this sport (“I like tough drills in practice because they help me to improve my skills” or “I prefer to play 3 on 3 with friends rather than practicing hard”). The relation between the achievement variables as assessed by WOFO and the motivational variables measured by the adapted DPMQ allowed us to suggest a model that clarifies the belonging and continuity to elite or local level groups. For this task we used paired athlete/outcome data to build a Decision Tree, a well known support tool for classification problems (Breiman et al., 1984). Decision Trees belong to the class of nonparametric, nonlinear, exploratory supervised learning algorithms which also include neural networks and support vector machines and can be used to make predictions on data, given that a training dataset for which the true outcomes are known is available. Decision Trees are however particularly well for our purpose given that besides being able to classify athletes into categories, they are also easily interpretable and provide information on the relative importance of each input variable to the final predicted classifications, something that cannot be straightforwardly found with neural networks and support vector machines. Furthermore, decision tree algorithms have the advantage of being able to handle mixed types of variables, being robust to outliers and being robust to irrelevant inputs. The first purpose of this study is to describe the achievement and motivation variables that can explain the belonging to an elite competitive level of young basketball players. The second goal is to suggest an explanatory model able to predict the engagement in deliberate practice. |