In recent years, extensive research has been carried out to analyse the physiological and biomechanical factors that characterise racket sport athletes (Manrique and González-Badillo, 2003), especially with tennis and squash players. There is however, limited data to assess which factors are desirable in competitive badminton (Huynh and Bedford, 2010; Manrique and González-Badillo, 2003). Despite its inclusion as an official sport in the 25th Olympic Games, research in the field of performance optimisation, mental and visual training, and skill acquisition for badminton remains scarce (Blomqvist et al., 2001; Huynh and Bedford, 2010; Manrique and González-Badillo, 2003). In attempting to train and improve badminton players, Huynh and Bedford, 2010 argue that the cognitive components of badminton must not be underemphasised. The authors suggest that in attempting to optimise skill proficiency, athletes need to incorporate a combination of both physical and cognitive aspects into their training program. They introduced a new visual based training (VBT) method of identifying and improving a badminton player’s reaction time and awareness: the Skills Acquisition Trainer for Badminton (SATB). This program however, is still fairly immature in nature, and additional studies and research are required to assess its accuracy. Previous research involving VBT has shown that the ability to detect and utilise advanced visual cues allows players to predict their opponent’s actions more accurately. A classic example of this can be found in Abernethy and Russell’s (1987) study regarding the differences between the ability of experts and novice to discriminate visual cues. The research suggested that novice badminton players were unable to detect information regarding advanced cue sources, which is the ability that provides experts with superior anticipatory skills. Specifically, the researchers stated that experts would utilise the visual cues from their opponent’s racket and arm placement to predict stroke direction and speed, whereas novices were only capable of extracting advance information from the racket itself. Renshaw and Fairweather, 2000 utilised a visual based method to examine expertise among cricket players by assessing verbal discrimination when faced with five different types of bowling deliveries. They showed that expert batters were more successful than novices in identifying different types of deliveries made by an expert wrist-spin bowler. The overall detection rates in this study were significantly different between national, regional and club cricket players. National players correctly identified 63% of deliveries, regional players identified 56%, and club players correctly identified 48% of overall deliveries. However, when examining this discrimination capability for types of delivery, the authors found that batters were less able to discriminate deliveries that were similar in nature, regardless of expertise. Renshaw and Fairweather, 2000 explained this poor discrimination ability due to the deliveries that were similar in nature to the legspin delivery (i.e. topspin and backspin). Similarly in badminton, the many different shot types used have similar appearances in execution, and may generally only be differentiated during the last few milliseconds prior to the racket making contact with the shuttle. These research studies lead us to predict that perceptual training early in an athlete’s skill development will prove beneficial for their anticipatory skills in the long run. However, this is not to say that VBT methods would be more efficient than, or that they should replace the standard training regimines of physical training. In a practical sense, adapting a perceptual strategy which emulates an expert will not bring a novice to that level simply by forcing the model onto them. From a dynamic systems approach, these types of visual imagery training would be insufficient (Renshaw and Fairweather, 2000) unless coupled with a form of physical practice. Ideally, it is the combination of both visual training and motor practice that will enhance overall perceptual performance. Furthermore, with the digital age constantly developing, and the nature in which Gen-Z children are raised and taught through digital means (Mitchell, 2008; Tapscott, 2008; Howe and Strauss, 2008), the use of a VBT method to train athletes (e.g. the SATB program) should prove not only effective but also stimulating for athletes of the future. Heazlewood and Keshishian, 2010 used perceptron neural networks in conjunction with discriminant analysis to identify the variables that characterise karate athletes into high and low performance groups. Their study revealed that both perceptron neural networks and discriminant function analysis yielded a high percentage of accuracy in categorising karate athletes into high and low performance groups. The authors of the present study attempted to replicate Heazlewood and Keshishian’s (2010) study, and apply both neural networks and discriminant function analysis to Huynh and Bedford’s (2010) SATB program. Derived from studies of brain functioning, the definition of a neural network varies depending upon the field in which it is being examined. In a statistical sense, a neural network applies to a loosely related family of models, characterised by a parameter space and flexible structure (SPSS Inc., 2007b). Neural networks are made up of numerous artificial neurons (modelled after biological neurons), each having their own associated weight. Buckland, 2002 states that the weights in most neural sets can be both positive and negative, therefore providing excitory or inhibitory influences to each input. As each input enters the nucleus, it’s multiplied by its weight. The nucleus then sums all these new input values which gives us the activation (refer to Figure 2). If the activation is greater than the threshold value , the neuron outputs a signal. If the activation is less than the threshold value, the neuron outputs zero. This is typically called a step function. If we consider the number of inputs a neuron can have as n, and the corresponding weights each input can have as w, then the equation for the activation value can be represented by: The Multilayer Perceptron (MLP) procedure produces a predictive model for one or more dependent variables based on the values of the predictor variables. What makes a multilayer perceptron unique is that each neuron uses a nonlinear activation function which was developed to model the frequency of action potentials, or firing, of biological neurons in the brain (Haykin, 1998). This function is modelled in several ways, but must always be normalizable and differentiable. The two main activation functions used in current applications are both sigmoids, and are described by: Function 2 is a hyperbolic tangent which ranges from -1 to 1, while function 3 is equivalent in shape but ranges from 0 to 1. Here yi is the output of the ith node (neuron) and vi is the weighted sum of the input synapses. Learning occurs in the perceptron by changing connection weights after each piece of data is processed, based on the amount of error in the output compared to the expected result (Haykin, 1998). This is an example of supervised learning, and is carried out through backpropagation, a generalization of the least mean squares algorithm in the linear perceptron. The error in output node j in the nth data point can be represented by ej(n) = dj(n) - yj(n), where d is the target value and y is the value produced by the perceptron. We then make corrections to the weights of the nodes based on those corrections which minimize the error in the entire output, given by: The advantage of utilising a neural network is that it can approximate a wide range of statistical models without requiring the researcher to hypothesise in advance certain relationships between the dependent and independent variables (Heazlewood and Keshishian, 2010; SPSS Inc., 2007b). Instead the form of the relationship is determined during the learning process. The trade-off for this flexibility is that the synaptic weights of a neural network are not easily interpretable. Thus, if you are trying to explain an underlying process that produces the relationships between the dependent and independent variables, it would be better to use discriminant analysis (Heazlewood and Keshishian, 2010). As explained by Heazlewood and Keshishian, 2010, discriminant analysis can be used to classify cases into the values of a categorical dependent variable, to predict group membership based on a linear combination of the interval variables. The procedure begins with a set of observations where both group membership and the values of the interval variables are known (Stockburger, 1998). The end result of the procedure is a model that allows prediction of group membership when only the interval variables are known. A second purpose of discriminant function analysis is an understanding of the data set, as a careful examination of the prediction model that results from the procedure can give insight into the relationship between group membership and the variables used to predict group membership (Stockburger, 1998). The aim of the present study was to compare the statistical ability of both neural networks and discriminant function analysis on the newly developed SATB (Huynh and Bedford, 2010). Using these statistical tools, we will attempt to identify the accuracy of the SATB in classifying badminton players into different skill level groups (e.g. beginner, intermediate, advanced). Finally, using these outcomes, in conjunction with the physiological and biomechanical variables of the participants, we will assess the authenticity and accuracy of the SATB and comment on the overall effectiveness of the VBT approach to training badminton athletes. |