Demonstration of validity is a fundamental part of science. The present study continued the validation of the BRUMS by extending validity for use with Malaysian athletes. This extends recent work that has extended validity of the BRUMS to different populations including a recent study that tested its validity among Malaysian athletes (Hashim et al., 2010). The present study extends this work by using a much larger sample, a feature of the research that is helpful when developing a dataset that could be used as the basis for norms. Results were supportive of recent research to the extent that the factor structure of the scale was largely supported and the weakest aspect of the model was the confusion scale, and particularly the item “Uncertain”. Recent research has argued that confusion is more of a cognitive rather than affective state and could be excluded from the measure on these grounds alone (Lane et al., 2007). Although confusion demonstrated strong relationships with other subscale scores, it is possible that feeling confused could be an antecedent or consequence of an emotional state rather than part of it. In a study that extended an extended version of the BRUMS (by including two additional factors: calmness and happiness), Lane et al., 2007 removed confusion on the basis of the arguments presented. Hashim et al., 2010 found the item uncertain demonstrated weak loadings with confusion, and cross-loaded it with tension. The nature of the item uncertain could feasible be a part of the tension scale, particularly as tension is also characterized by feeling nervous and worried. Tension is closely related to anxiety and uncertainty about goal pursuit is central to this concept. It should be noted that other items in the subscale have less affective tone (mixed-up, muddled, and confused). It should be noted that the research strategy was to test the hypothesized six-factor model originally supported by Terry et al., 1999. Recent research tested the Malay version of the BRUMS against alternative models such as a single and two-factor model (see Hashim et al., 2010). This approach was not followed in the present study as it is argued that combining the unpleasant states in the POMS into a single factor can result in a loss of information. For example, an athlete might feel angry but not fatigued if the anger is a consequence of an argument with a coach. Equally, an athlete might feel fatigued as a consequence of a hard training session but not report feeling anger. We suggest that researchers and practitioners alike use the independent subscales, even though, as in the case of the present study, they can be strongly interrelated. In terms of the direction of inter-correlations between subscales, results are consistent with those reported by Terry et al. (1999, 2003). However, inter-correlations among BRUMS subscales are stronger than those reported by Terry et al, an observation that can be applied to each correlation. It should be noted that Terry et al. found vigor did not relate significantly to confusion, anger and tension whereas intercorrelations between in the present study showed an inverse relationship. Further, in the CFA conducted by Terry et al, the correlation between vigor, anger, confusion and tension was set to zero. A comparison of descriptive statistics with those reported by Terry et al demonstrates that participants in the present study reported significantly higher scores of depressed mood. Depressed mood has been shown to influence the intensity of other mood states and strengthen the correlation among independent mood states (Lane, 2007). Lane, 2007 summarized studies that tested the Lane and Terry, 2000 model that show that when participants report symptoms of depression, the correlation between independent mood states increases; that is, if a participant reports feeling depressed, he also tends to report feeling confused, angry and tense. However, before applying this logic to the present study it is important to consider possible cultural differences. It is possible that Malaysian athletes report their feelings on subscales such as depression more freely than those in a Western country (see Mastor et al., 2000). Lane and Terry, 2000 reported that the norm for the depression scale of the BRUMS in Western countries is zero; that is, participants report the lowest score possible for all items. This representation of their feelings might be accurate, but it is possible that an individual will recognize that reporting to feel depressed is not consistent with a cultural norm that advocates feeling high scores for pleasant emotions such as excitement (i.e., those items on the vigor scale). We suggest that future research is needed to investigate processes underlying the nature of self-report; that is explore how individuals process information in order to rate how they are feeling. Two key areas are important here; the first is the extent to which people have accurate insights to how they are feeling. Evidence suggests that people vary in their ability to recall and assess emotion related experiences (Petrides et al., 2007). We suggest that interviews with might offer some insight whether an individual is genuinely in the way reported in the scale. The second is that research should look to explore the meaning people attribute to such emotions. Recent research in sport psychology has started to emphasize the importance of exploring cultural factors (Schinke and Hanrahan, 2009). We suggest that this be extended to examining the nature of emotions in different cultures and the research should begin with examining validity issues as validity is such an important aspect of the scientific process. The importance of exploring cultural factors is a growing line of investigation in sport psychology (see Schinke and Hanrahan, 2009; Terry, 2009). Terry, 2009 suggested that the belief systems of different cultures vary and influence client-practitioner interactions and as such could influence how emotions are expressed and regulated. In fact, there has been a recent growth in research investigating the influence of culture on emotions and emotion regulation (Mesquita and Albert, 2007). The way in which people appraise the environment or stimuli is proposed to be important in subsequent emotional experiences (Mesquita and Albert, 2007). At least three suggestions emerge for researchers and practitioners from the present study. The first is that researchers and practitioners alike can use the BRUMS with greater confidence. The second is that descriptive statistics can be used to produce normative data which can help practitioners interpret raw scores. A limitation of the sport psychology literature is that few scales have normative data on which to interpret scores despite the development of norms being advocated as best practice (Anastasi and Urbina, 1997). The third is that research should use qualitative methods to explore the nature of emotions and mood states within each culture. In conclusion, findings offer some support for the validity of the BRUMS for use with Malaysian athletes and normative data is present. We suggest future research should look to explore the nature of emotion and mood in different cultures. |