Enjoyment has been identified as a significant factor underlying exercise motivation for children and adolescents to maintain their positive engagement in both PA and PE (Prochaska et al., 2003; Sallis, Prochaska and Taylor, 2000; Wallhead and Buckworth, 2004; Yli-Piipari et al., 2009). However, previous studies have revealed that adolescents' PA decreases across the secondary school years (Corbin, Pangrazi and Le-Masurier, 2004; Currie et al., 2008; Yli-Piipari, 2011). Therefore, it is important to understand how motivational climate, perceived physical competence and exercise motivation influence enjoyment in school PE. The aim of this study was to examine whether motivational climate, perceived physical competence, and exercise motivation variables predict enjoyment in school PE. It was hypothesized that the perception of task-involving climate at Grade 7 would be a positive predictor of PE enjoyment at Grade 9 via perceived physical competence and intrinsic motivation. Additionally, it was hypothesized that ego-involving climate at Grade 7 would be a negative predictor of PE enjoyment at Grade 9 via perceived physical competence and extrinsic motivation. This study was the first, in which the effects of social-cognitive motivational factors (i.e. motivational climate, perceived physical competence, and intrinsic motivation) on PE enjoyment were analysed across the secondary school years among the same sample of adolescents. A large sample of 639 students were monitored across their entire involvement at secondary school and assessed at several key measurement points. Previous studies have been designed as available cohort investigations with samples changing across various time periods (e.g. Neumark-Sztainer, 2003) or relatively short-term interventions (e.g. Digelidis et al., 2003; Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004). The findings of the current study support existing suggestions of Vallerand's (1997) model in which social factors mediated by a psychological mediator, and exercise motivation are related to positive consequences in the PE context such as fundamental movement skills (Kalaja et al., 2009), intentions to participate in optional PE courses (Ntoumanis, 2005), self-regulated motivation (Ommundsen and Eikanger-Kvalo, 2007), and concentration levels in PE (Standage, Duda and Ntoumanis, 2005). The hypothesized motivational model revealed a non-acceptable fit for the data with girls and boys together. In previous studies, PE enjoyment (Carroll and Loumidis, 2001), perceived physical competence (Carroll and Loumidis, 2001; Fairclough, 2003), extrinsic motivation (Yli-Piipari, 2011), and ego-involving motivational climate (Moreno-Murcia et al., 2011) have been shown to have statistically significant gender differences. Therefore, we used gender as a grouping variable in subsequent path analyses. As hypothesized, task-involving motivational climate predicted PE enjoyment via perceived physical competence and intrinsic motivation in both girls and boys. In particular, these results supported previous findings associated with the self- determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985; 1991; 2000) and the achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1989) as demonstrated by the linking of task-involving climate in PE to perceived physical competence (Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004), intrinsic motivation (Digelidis and Papaioannou, 1999), and PE enjoyment (Digelidis and Papaioannou, 2002; Soini, 2006; Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004). The current results provide continuing support for the investigation of the Vallerand's model in the PE setting, and highlights that motivational climate is an area that requires continuing evaluation as a contributing factor in improving PE teaching. In contrast, for the hypothesized model, ego-involving motivational climate had poor fit with the psychological outcomes in PE. Ego-involving climate did not fit either the data for the girls or boys. This finding did not support the model of Vallerand, 1997, as PE lessons based on ego-involving motivational climate did not significantly influence on the level of PE enjoyment. This finding is similar to the results of the British study (Ntoumanis et al., 2005), in which they found that task-involving motivational climate was more conducive to the satisfaction of the three psychological needs compared to an ego-involving climate. Standage, Duda and Ntoumanis, 2005 evidenced also that a lesser extent of perceptions of mastery climate positively impacted hypothesized mediating variables to foster self-determined motivation. Either extrinsic motivation did not fit into the hypothesized model, dissimilarly with previous findings of Ntoumanis, 2002 and Standage, Duda and Ntoumanis, 2005, in which non-autonomous types of motivation have been shown to be related to negative outcomes in PE. The possible reason for ego-involving climate and extrinsic motivation being removed from the hypothesized model might be the setting with several measurement points. Although previous studies (Kalaja et al., 2009; Ntoumanis, 2005; Ommundsen and Eikanger-Kvalo, 2007; Standage, Duda and Ntoumanis, 2005) have lent some support for the Vallerand's model (1997), most of them have not been conducted using several measurement points through entire secondary school years. The results also showed that task-involving motivational climate in PE lessons at Grade 7 had a strong association with PE enjoyment via perceived physical competence and intrinsic motivation at Grade 9 for both girls and boys. The few intervention studies previously reported have shown that task-involving motivational climate has positive effects on perceived physical competence (Weigand and Burton, 2002), exercise motivation (Jaakkola, 2002), and PE enjoyment (Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004). Similarly, previous research has revealed that perceived physical competence is strongly connected with both task-involving climate in PE (Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004) and enjoyment in PE (Carroll and Loumidis, 2001; Fairclough, 2003). The finding of this study in conjunction with previous findings are in line with the suggestion of Ferrer-Caja and Weiss, 2000 that events that provide opportunities to satisfy an individual's need to perceive competence in dealing with the contextual motivational climate will maximize intrinsic motivation, and further enjoyment. Furthermore, Ferrer-Caja and Weiss, 2000 proposed that adolescents who report stronger beliefs about their physical competencies are more likely to enjoy the activity than adolescents who report lower levels of physical competence. In accordance with previous research, intrinsic motivation is associated with positive behavioral consequences such as greater enjoyment in PE (Ntoumanis, 2001; Ryan and Deci, 2007; Yli-Piipari, 2011). Additionally, PE enjoyment was explained rather strongly by task-involving climate, perceived physical competence, and intrinsic motivation among both girls and boys. These findings were congruent with the self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985; 1991; 2000) and the achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1989), as enjoyment has been previously shown to be positively related to cognitions, such as intrinsic motivation (Vallerand, 1997; Yli-Piipari et al., 2009), perceptions of competence (Hashim, Grove and Whipp, 2008; Wankel, 1997), and task-involving motivational climate (Digelidis and Papaioannou, 2002). An interesting fact related to the current results was that PE enjoyment among girls was better explained by task-involving climate via perceived physical competence, and intrinsic motivation than for the boys. This finding is dissimilar with the findings of Carroll and Loumidis, 2001, as they found that boys scored higher than girls on enjoyment in a sample of British Grade 6 students. Therefore, task-involving motivational climate could be used as a relevant method to promote perceived competence, intrinsic motivation, and further enjoyment in school PE, especially among girls. From the point of view of PE enjoyment, school physical education could be most effective if based on task-involving motivational climate, in which the main objective is increasing students' perceived physical competence, intrinsic motivation, and further enjoyment. In previous interventions, based on the TARGET model of Epstein, 1989, task-involving methods have been shown to be valid to improve positive consequences in school PE. In the TARGET model, task- and ego-involving climate consist of certain motivational structures. These structures construct the teaching model, in which task (design of learning activities), authority (locus of decision-making), recognition (criteria for rewards), grouping (homogenous or heterogeneous ability), evaluation (criteria for success or failure), and timing (pace of instructions) create the TARGET acronym. Basically, teachers can manipulate these six features to influence the motivational climate in their PE classes to reflect either task- or ego-involving motivational climate. For instance, Wallhead and Ntoumanis, 2004 found in their intervention that PE enjoyment increased in conjunction with perceived effort, perceived competence, and goal orientation when a) students chose personal skill practices from a range of offered practices, b) students were responsible for setting up equipment, c) recognition was based on individual progress, d) student worked together within same small cooperative group structure, e) student-coaches emphasized individual improvement in order to benefit team performance goals, and f) during lessons students often dictated the rate of progression through specific practices. Specifically, during the intervention, students led warm-ups, took responsibility for refereeing and the choice of tactics and team strategies, and were responsible for selecting individuals to fulfill each role of coach, referee, captain, and scorer. The results of the current study and previous practical findings support task-involving teaching methods to promote adolescent's PE enjoyment through secondary school years among both girls and boys. A key limitation of this study is related to the use of subjective scales to evaluate the main variables. The truthfulness and accuracy of self-report measures may be compromised because some health and well-being behaviors such as PA are difficult to recall and may also be so sensitive that respondents are reluctant to provide exact details. In addition, adolescents may purposely under-report or over-report some health and well-being behaviors because they believe engaging in these behaviors is socially undesirable or desirable (Brener, Billy and Grady, 2003). A further limitation of the study is the number of imputed missing values for the PE enjoyment variable. However, imputation was used as a method to save a remarkable part of the original data following the procedure proposed by Allison, 2002, and Widaman, 2006. Future research should be directed towards the undertaking of additional longitudinal studies about the development of PE enjoyment from childhood to late adolescence, and the social-cognitive motivational processes behind PA engagement in PE. Specifically, these types of investigations could generate valuable evidence about the critical transition periods from elementary school to secondary school and from secondary to upper secondary or vocational school. This information could be utilized in various practical applications, such as PE teacher training and the professional development of existing PE teachers by providing a clearer understanding of the development of adolescents' PA patterns and their motivational determinants. |