Cluster 4 (medium peak, slow decay) was significantly associated with the highest performance improvement. In cluster 4, the total training load was reduced from 55% of the most efficient cluster in OP (cluster 2 OP, 84 ± 17; 81 ± 22, 80 ± 19% mean TTL). This 55% training load decrement is in accordance with the 40-60% values generally suggested in the literature (Bosquet et al., 2007, Houmard and Johns, 1994; Johns et al., 1992; Mujika et al., 1996a; 1996b). Swimming economy has been reported to improve as a result of training load reduction in taper (D’Acquisto et al., 1992). Other studies have also shown that increases in maximal heart rate (Houmard and Johns, 1994), hemoglobin and hematocrit values (Yamamoto et al., 1988), and muscular power (Costill et al., 1985; 1991; Johns et al., 1992) are positively related to reduced training volume during taper. In the most efficient pattern during TP (cluster 4, medium peak, slow decay), the 55% reduction in the total training load of pre-taper values was achieved through a 37% decrease in low-intensity training (training below the lactic threshold), a 49% decrease in high-intensity training (above the lactic threshold), and a 95% decrease in strength training. The 37% decrease in low-intensity training was less than the usual decline recommended in taper studies, which suggest performance improvement for a 60% decrement in low-intensity training (Bosquet et al., 2007; Mujika and Padilla, 2003; Pyne et al., 2009). The 49% decrease in high-intensity training (above the lactic threshold) in the most effective training pattern (cluster 4) is in line with most earlier studies, which demonstrated the paramount importance of training intensity in maintaining the training-induced adaptations during periods of reduced training (Bosquet et al., 2007; Houmard and Johns, 1994; Mujika and Padilla, 2003; Pyne et al., 2009). These studies showed that intensity training had a fundamental role in preserving the physiological adaptations obtained during earlier periods of intensity training. However, the relationship between performance improvement and the decreases in training intensity during TP in the present study agrees with other reports suggesting the importance of decreasing training intensity during the final weeks preceding major events (Hellard et al., 2005; Mujika et al., 1996a; Van Handel et al., 1988). For instance, Van Handel and co-workers (1988) studied physiological and performance changes in elite swimmers performing 20 days of taper, with training volume dropping from 10,000-12,000 m to 2,000-3,000 m·day-1 while training intensity was held constant. They suggested that training intensity should also be reduced to further optimize the effects of taper, allowing adequate rest and recovery. Mujika et al., 1996a reported a 2.94 ± 1.51% and 3.18 ± 1.70% improvement in performances during three taper periods lasting three and four weeks, respectively, in the course of which significant reductions were observed in the weekly distance swum in the high-intensity training zones. It has also been pointed out that excessive training at high intensity could lead to the deterioration of stroking parameters (particularly, stroke length) as stroke mechanics deteriorate at speeds above the anaerobic threshold as a consequence of local muscular fatigue (Dekerle et al., 2005; Toussaint et al., 2006). Therefore, it could be speculated that, in swimming, the amount of intensity training during taper needs to be optimized in order to maintain the training-induced adaptations acquired during the preceding overload training periods, while maintaining a high efficient swimming technique. Cluster 3 (medium training load, low decay), which showed the smallest decrease in the training load, was significantly associated with the poorest performance improvement during taper. An insufficient decrement in training load probably did not permit the biological and psychological stress of the overload training period to resolve (Bosquet et al., 2007; Mujika et al., 1996a, Mujika and Padilla, 2003; Thomas and Busso, 2005; Thomas et al., 2008). A novel finding of this exploratory study on training periodization is that the optimal training design for the pre-taper and taper periods gradually changed over the course of the athletes’ careers. From the 1st to the 3rd season, higher performance improvements were associated with lower training loads during the overload training period (the greater the training distance, the smaller the difference was between the performances at the major and preparatory events). Furthermore, training-load maintenance during taper was associated with greater improvements during the first three competitive seasons (the lower the difference between the overload and taper periods in the amount of training at low intensity, the greater the improvement was in performance after taper). From the 4th competitive season, the training effects were progressively reversed. High training loads during the overload period followed by a sharp decrease in the above-mentioned variables, as well as in total training load during taper, led to greater performance improvement. These results suggest that training load responses depend on the years of exposure to intensive training (Avalos et al., 2003; Busso et al., 1997). In line with this suggestion, Avalos et al., 2003 modeled the training-performance response relationship in 13 elite swimmers over three seasons and demonstrated that reactions to mid- and long-term training were significantly modified between the 1st and 3rd season. The same training load for the three seasons led to a negligible decrease in the mid-term performance (2-3 weeks before the competition) and a decrease in the long-term training period (4-6 weeks before the competition). For the taper periods directly preceding major events, the results of the present study confirmed the results of Busso and co-workers (Busso et al., 1997, Busso et al., 2002), who reported an increase over time in the magnitude and duration of fatigue induced by a single training bout. First, in high-level athletes who have been training intensively for many years, further progress in physiological adaptations and performance assumes continued and progressive increases in training loads (Avalos et al., 2003; Gaskill et al., 1999; Mujika et al., 2002; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000b; Thomas et al., 2008). Nevertheless, these athletes require longer recovery times (Avalos et al., 2003; Gaskill et al., 1999; Mujika et al., 2002; Thomas and Busso, 2005; Thomas et al., 2008). Last, the present study suggests that at the beginning of intensive swimming practice, the optimal design for young swimmers should basically consist of a continued training load distribution. Conversely, after several years of athletic career, training volume should be increased during the overload training periods and decreased during the taper periods, with the total amount of high-intensity training also proportionately decreased (i.e., the relative amount of high-intensity training is maintained or increased). Significant inter-individual variability, however, suggests that these general training recommendations should be adapted so that a personal model is constructed for each subject, as advocated in most studies of the training-performance relationship (Avalos et al., 2003; Hellard et al., 2005; Mujika et al., 1996a; 1996b; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000a; 2000b). One limitation of this study was the three-week taper period, as the consensus is that this period should vary from one to four weeks, depending on age, sex, swimming specialty, and the type of overload training conducted during the preceding period (Avalos et al., 2003; Mujika et al., 1996a; 2002; Thomas et al., 2008). However, in observational studies with many subjects followed over many years, it is not possible to experimentally vary the competition periods. Most countries program preparatory competitions three weeks before the major competitive events, as reported by Mujika and his team in 2002. Another limitation of the study concerns the calculation of the change in performance. One could argue that the greatest improvement with the final reduction in training does not necessarily mean reaching the highest performance after the taper. For example, Thomas and his colleagues used computer simulations and showed that a greater overload before taper could lead to a greater decrement in performance before taper and thus enhance the increase with taper (Thomas et al., 2008). The results of the analyses of our observational data indicate that such cases are infrequent. Indeed, in the 85 periods we studied, 53 of the performances during the National Championships were the best performances during the winter period, which runs from September to the National Championships. Twenty-six of the best winter performances were during the preparatory competition three weeks earlier, suggesting that top form was reached too early. Last, for six of the periods, the performance reached during the preparatory period was very low, which may have reflected a training overload and/or a medical problem. |