Research article - (2014)13, 266 - 270 |
An Enjoyable Distraction During Exercise Augments the Positive Effects of Exercise on Mood |
Gregory J. Privitera, Danielle E. Antonelli, Abigail L. Szal |
Key words: Distraction, exercise, aerobic, mood, enjoyment |
Key Points |
|
|
|
Participants |
A sample of 84 undergraduate student volunteers (52 women, 32 men) was recruited through university classroom visits and sign-up sheets. Participant sample characteristics were (M±SD) age (19.8 ± 0.9 years), weight (73 ± 6.4 kg), height (1.77 ± 0.05 m), and BMI (24.8 ± 3.2 kg·m-2). The 84 participants passed an initial screening used to determine if individuals qualified to participate. All participants were light to moderate exercisers (from 0.5 to 1.5 hours of exercise per week), and were non-smokers who were in general good health with no physical or physician/doctor diagnosed medical conditions including pregnancy, and no dietary or exercise restrictions. Also, all participants ate within the range of 1 to 2 hours prior to the study to control for differences in hunger states, which can otherwise influence changes in affect/mood and arousal (Frank et al., |
Measures and equipment |
Procedures |
The study was split into two phases: A grouping phase to assign participants to groups ( Participants in the exercise groups then began their exercise by walking on a treadmill at the standardized speed of 3.6 mph for 10 min (walking speed). According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, walking is the most popular aerobic physical exercise among adults (CDC, 2013). For this reason, we chose a walking exercise at a “brisk” walking pace (1) to hold constant the intensity of the exercise, and (2) to use a type of low-exertion physical exercise (i.e., walking) that is most popular among adults who exercise. Participants in the ED + Exercise, the NED + Exercise, and the ED Only groups watched the same video clip of “Two and a Half Men” during the 10 min. Participants who did not exercise (ED Only group) stood on the treadmill while watching the video clip so as to be treated the same as participants in the exercise groups (minus actually exercising). All participants were told only to look forward (toward the television screen) or to look down (to check their footing); all participants followed these instructions. The television screen was turned off during exercise for participants in the Exercise Only group. Participant heart rate was recorded in all groups at the start of the exercise/10 min (baseline) and in 2 min intervals for 10 min to check the exercise intensity levels in the exercise and no exercise groups (Karvonen and Vuorimaa, |
Data analyses |
A one-way between-subjects ANOVA was computed with groups as the between-subjects factor and mood and arousal ratings as the dependent variables. Tukey’s HSD was used as the post hoc test. Sex was also included as a factor, but removed when it showed no significance with the effects reported here. To check if changes in mood were evident within each group, 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were drawn for differences in mood and arousal scores. The null hypothesis under evaluation for each group was that the mean difference score in the population was zero. A change in mood or arousal was identified if the 95% CI did not envelop zero. To check if participants in each exercise group showed similar exertion during exercise and compared to the no exercise control group, a two-way mixed factorial ANOVA was also computed with heart rate as the dependent variable. Time (Baseline, 2, 4, 6, 8, and 10 min) was the within-subjects factor and groups were the between-subjects factor. All tests were conducted at a .05 level of significance. |
|
|
With mood as the dependent variable, an effect of groups was significant, F(3, 80) = 10.89, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.29. Post hoc tests showed that compared to all other groups, participants in Group ED + Exercise showed the largest increase in the intensity of pleasant mood post-exercise (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.02 for all tests), as shown in The factorial ANOVA showed that heart rate significantly increased over time during the exercise/10 min, F(5, 400) = 108.95, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.58, and that this change depended on group assignment, F(15, 400) = 10.80, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.29. As shown in |
|
|
The hypothesis that an enjoyable distraction during exercise will enhance mood post-exercise was tested. The results presented here show support for this hypothesis by showing an augmentation effect of an enjoyable distraction on mood: While exercise alone was sufficient to increase the intensity of pleasant mood; combining exercise with an enjoyable distraction resulted in a significantly greater increase in pleasant mood compared to exercise alone. Participants watching the same show during exercise but who rated it as not enjoyable and those who did not exercise showed no changes in mood pre- to post-exercise. Hence, exercise alone increased the intensity of pleasant mood, and an enjoyable distraction during exercise significantly augmented pleasant mood changes compared to exercise alone. Some alternative explanations can be eliminated because of the controls included in the design. Heart rate data confirmed that the intensity of the exercise was the same for all exercise groups. Therefore, differences To date, there has been rather inconsistent reporting of how distractions during exercise contribute to pleasant mood changes post-exercise (Russell et al., Because of the nature of the study design, the type of distraction was a quasi-independent variable. Participants selected themselves to each distraction type group based on their responses to ranking their favorite television shows. Hence, one possible limitation to this study is selection differences among people who enjoy the show “Two and a Half Men” and those who do not enjoy the show. Also, because only one genre of video was used (i.e., a comedy), it cannot be determined if other types of distractions (e.g., dramas, action films) will have a similar effect on pleasant mood during exercise. Future studies can investigate these possibilities, by selecting participants to experience many different types and genres of distractions during exercise to identify the generality of the effects reported here. More studies are certainly needed to identify the specific mechanisms in a distraction that lead to or cause the increased intensity of pleasant mood (other than individual differences). Still, these results suggest that accounting for the enjoyment of a distraction type in future studies can nevertheless increase the sensitivity of research designs used to detect changes in post-exercise mood. Also, the results presented here demonstrate that an enjoyable distraction during exercise can increase pleasant mood post-exercise, which could have possible therapeutic effects in a gym setting among college-aged samples. |
|
|
The results presented here show evidence for one possible factor contributing to the effectiveness of distractions on exercise: the enjoyment of the distraction. The results show support for the conclusion that watching an enjoyable distraction during exercise can augment the positive effects of exercise on mood. Future studies that manipulate distractions during exercise should account for this factor to increase the sensitivity of their research designs to detect changes in mood following exercise. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|