This cross-cultural adaptation study provided sufficient evidence for the validity and reliability of the Spanish version of the PEAS (PEAS-ESP), which can be recommended with confidence for future doping studies among Spanish speaking athletes and their entourage. Internal consistency values, ranging between acceptable to very good, indicated good reliability of the PEAS-ESP across several samples. The one-week test-retest reliability suggested that PEAS-ESP measures a relatively stable construct. However, the significant difference between the first and the second administration of the PEAS-ESP was in line with Petróczi and Aidman (2009) who justified this using the theory of dynamics attitudes (Eiser, 1994) and the attitudes priming model (Fazio, 1995). Taking everything into account, it is reasonable that in the second administration participants gave more socially desirable responses because it is possible that the first administration of the questionnaire may have sensitized the participants. CFA results confirmed the factorial structure similar to the original version in all but one sample. The correlations between the errors terms of items 4, 11 and 12 suggested the possible presence of an additional, albeit weak, latent factor. Upon closer investigation of these items, it became apparent that all three items refer to recreational drug use in sport context. Thus it is conceivable that the large sample size in this particular study afforded the emergence of an additional latent factor related to recreational drugs in general or in sport that was not intended to be measured by the PEAS but potentially confounded the measures taken by these particular items on doping. This latent factor could have remained hidden previously as most studies utilised a considerably small samples and in any case, relatively non-significant as neither Velicer’s or Horns’ tests indicated the presence of more than one factor. Further research is required to fully explore the potential confounding effect of an attitude toward recreational drugs in doping context. In addition, according to Hooper et al., (2008), we suggest that limitations of chi square test must be taken into account when interpreting results from this particularly large sample of over 2000 participants. Firstly, this test assumes multivariate normality whereas in our study, most samples are non-normal. Severe deviations from normality may result in model rejections even when the model is properly specified; secondly, because the chi-square statistic is in essence a statistical significance test it is sensitive to sample size leading to the model nearly always being rejected when large samples are used. In addition to its apparent validity, it was also found evidence favorable to construct and convergent validity. It was expected that self-admitted doping users would show higher PEAS scores and, consequently, a more lenient attitude toward doping. In 5 of the 8 analysed samples significant differences were found between confessed users and non-users (p < 0.05). In the other 3 samples in which statistical difference was not reached, the small sample size of users could be the reason. We suggest that “practical differences” (Atkinson, 2003) were observed in all comparisons (always higher scores for users), despite not always reaching statistical differences. This is in line with other studies, where confessed doping users, as expected, scored significantly higher on the PEAS when compared with those who reported no use of banned drugs (Petróczi and Aidman, 2009; Uvacsek et al., 2011) Regarding relationship between self-esteem and attitude towards doping, we found a significant negative correlation in both samples. Consequently, although this relationship is weak, the fact that similar data were obtained one year later with a smaller sample size could reinforce the idea that this relationship exists and is consistent. The relationship between self-esteem and attitudes toward doping could be the function of the driving forces behind doping use (i.e. aesthetic vs. performance). We suggested that this relationship may be relatively weak because the sample characteristics. Amateur cyclists most likely looked for other aims such as performance improvement instead of appearance. Moreover, Spearman’s correlations from users in relation to anti-doping self-efficacy and doping attitude could support the hypothesis that, at least in part, participants’ self-esteem and self-efficacy are related to attitudes towards doping. Doping behaviour research would benefit from further research in this under-researched aspect in doping. In relation to perceived descriptive norms, it seems participants who admitted using prohibited performance-enhancing substances believe that a higher percentage of the others are taking banned substances; and exhibited a more positive attitude toward doping on the PEAS-ESP scale. This phenomenon, called “false consensus effect” (Ross, Greene, & House, 1977) has been shown in doping before (Petróczi, Mazanov, Nepusz, Backhouse, & Naughton, 2008). Similarly, in the study of Uvacsek et al. (2011), domain specificity of this effect was evidenced. Contrary to previous research, gender did not have an effect on doping attitudes, but age did. Participants under 35 years of age (younger than limit age to be competitive in sport which we established) showed more permissive attitudes towards doping in the analysed sample what could be because they are supposed to be more competitive and more focused on performance in comparison to the oldest. From a doping prevention point of view, PEAS could be used as a standard measurement instrument to assess attitudes towards doping so that data were reliable and valid, and practical applications could be developed efficiently (Mandic et al., 2013). In addition, PEAS could be complemented with other tools such as interviews (Lentillon-Kaestner et al., 2012), implicit association tests (James et al., 2010) or ideally biomedical tests (Morente-Sanchez and Zabala, 2013). We suggested that Sport sciences researchers could play an important role in the fight against doping applying these researching methods to assess the current situation deeply in order to design, consequently, specific programs and other activities for doping prevention (Kisaalita and Robinson, 2014). |