Research article - (2015)14, 340 - 346 |
The Situated Management of Safety during Risky Sport: Learning from Skydivers’ Courses of Experience |
Sara Mohamed, Vincent Favrod, Roberta Antonini Philippe, Denis Hauw |
Key words: Skydiving, situated action, activity, meaning, training, involvement, subjective experience, education |
Key Points |
|
|
|
Participants |
Four male skydivers holding the Swiss license volunteered to participate in this study with an average age was 30.25 years ( |
Data collection |
The investigation analyzed four jump training sessions. Each skydiver was followed by the same investigator from the preparation of his material to landing and packing of the parachute. All had chosen their jump program without any restrictions from the investigator: three of them were freefall jumps (René, Laurent, Bertrand) and one was a wingsuit jump (Eric). Two types of data were collected: (a) videotapes of the skydivers' activity from jump preparation to landing and (b) video-recorded and transcribed verbalizations and commentaries elicited post-activity during self-confrontation interviews. The skydivers’ activity was recorded by the second author using a digital helmet-mounted camera to collect video data, even during the jump. After the jump, the participants viewed the videotape of their own activity during the self-confrontation interview (Theureau, |
Data analysis |
The identification and labeling of the actions were accomplished on the basis of (a) the activity videotapes and (b) the participants’ verbalizations. We used an action verb followed by a direct object, an adverb, or another complement (e.g., “gets into a good position for exiting the plane,” “looks at other skydivers to ensure safety distance”). The label reflected the responses to a number of questions about the skydivers’ activity in relation to the action as it appeared in the video recordings and self-confrontation data. Thus, each label for an elementary unit of meaning grouped together the action and the meaningful part of it and was then called an “action unit.” After identifying an action unit, we isolated the Representamen (i.e., what the athletes were focused on) and the Interpretant (i.e., what they knew, understood or felt about the situation). Then, we extracted the action units that contained safety elements. They were included in safety sequences identified and labeled by finding relationships between the action units and their underlying constituents. Each safety sequence was made up of action units that formed a coherent chain around a meaningful safety theme for the skydivers (e.g., the sequence “To ensure a safe landing” was composed of action units that aimed at ensuring a landing in a free area, at reasonable distance from the other skydivers and with a controlled speed of approach). The sequences were labeled with an infinitive verb followed by a direct object in order to take into account the dynamics of action unit generation in the stream of activity and reflect the skydivers' meaningful safety concerns in the situation. The timing of each action unit per activity was determined from the chronological data recorded during the self-confrontation interviews: each unit of action was timed using the corresponding moment identified by the skydiver. Then, the duration of each action and sequence were delimited by inference. The safety sequences identified in all the skydivers’ courses of action were compared, respecting the time match in order to identify interpersonal and individual components. After data collection, the coding process was checked for inter-reliability (the agreement rate ranged from 78% to 90% between coders for the different components), which was sufficiently high (i.e., higher than 0.70, Van Someren et al., |
|
|
The comparison of the skydivers’ courses of experience showed seven typical sequences containing safety concerns over the course of the skydiving experience. They were labeled “To check the material during preparation,” “To feel prepared and safe for the jump as the plane gains altitude,” “To use the time of freefall,” “To deploy the parachute,” “To fly safely,” “To ensure a safe landing” and “To organize the structured packing of the parachute.” |
To check the material during preparation |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ concerns were preparing the materials and checking their condition in terms of safety. |
To feel prepared and safe for the jump as the plane gains altitude |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ concerns shifted from relative anxiety to the feeling of being ready to jump. |
To use the time of freefall |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ concerns were focused on how to move in the air during this part of the flight starting from the exiting of the plane. There were many possible actions (e.g., to draw closer to other skydivers, to increase the speed, to change position, and so on) and the safety concerns here were connected to maintaining a safe distance, the speed of other skydivers, the normality of their own position in the freefall and the beep of the altisound that ended the sequence. |
To deploy the parachute |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ safety concerns were focused on the shift from a freefall situation to a fall with a balanced parachute. The main safety concern was the correct opening of the parachute as described in |
To fly safely |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ safety concerns were focused on managing the flight space in relation to the other skydivers and heading toward the landing area. The first concern was linked to safety because a collision would have very serious consequences. The second was associated with the anticipation of a good landing and thus with a form of safety. |
To ensure a safe landing |
This sequence comprised action units in which the skydivers’ safety concerns were focused on how to manage a smooth landing. As described in |
To organize the structured packing of the parachute |
This sequence comprised the action units of packing the parachute. The skydivers remained very concentrated during this period and followed a standardized and regulated procedure to ensure the safety for future jumps, as shown in |
|
|
The aim of this study was to analyze how safety is managed in a training situation during a high-risk sport. The results showed that the activity could be broken down into seven sequences in which safety was in the skydivers’ field of concerns. These sequences included various components like the material, environment, other skydivers and personal feelings. These components were not omnipresent concerns as they appeared at different periods in different ways. For example, safety concerns during the flight before the jump were targeted to personal involvement and material, while during the freefall the concerns were related to others and personal feelings. These results suggest that safety management emerged as multimodal concerns regularly distributed over the time of the activity. This seems to correspond to a form of the discontinuous sequences described by Theureau ( The order of these sequences indicated well-defined safety management. The safety concerns shifted from checking all personal material to concentration during the flight in the aircraft, from checking the unfolding of the situation at the beginning of the jump (freefall) to making sure that the main parachute would open correctly and then successively controlling the distance from others during the flight and the speed of landing, and finally ensuring great care in packing the parachute. This management was thus situated, linked to the needs at the very moment Our results also indicated that these discontinuous sequences corresponded to various forms of involvement for the skydivers. The first one was related to an increase in carefulness and rigor dedicated to meaningful elements. For example, the skydivers showed great care and rigor with the material during the sequence “To check the material during preparation.” The second one was an extreme concentration. This was observed during the sequence “To feel prepared and safe for the jump while the plane gains altitude” when skydivers moved from anxiety to intense concentration. The third involvement was the openness to specific events. This was observed during the flight after the freefall and was related to the other skydivers. These three types of involvement suggest that safety was not merely a question of respecting rules, but was actually a way to enact them with a highly specific subjective experience that engaged the entire body and a particular mindset in relation to an environmental setting. These results indicated what it meant to “be there” in terms of safety concerns (e.g., Clark, The results also described the evolving linkage between actions and situations as skydivers proceed through their jumps and suggest that safety is managed not only as discontinuous sequences, but also as the outcome of the practitioner’s cumulative global safety concerns. This cumulative effect has been observed for other types of sports activity. For example, in table tennis, Seve et al. ( It should be noted that there were times when safety concerns identified were managed similarly by all participants, and other times when there were clear differences. This could be interpreted as a certain irregularity in the critical need for safety concerns, such as we observed for trampolining, with three patterns of activity (Hauw and Durand, This study is presented as a case report, and it is thus impossible to generalize the results we obtained to the whole population of skydivers. In addition, other limitations to this study should to be underlined. Using a retrospective design presented methodological challenges, and the validity of building new meaning as someone explains their past experience may be questioned. However, in an enactive and situated paradigm, traces of past activity are presented to the study participants during the self-confrontation interview in order to stimulate a re-enactment process (Hauw, |
|
|
Our study revealed the dynamical characteristics of practical knowledge about skydiving safety, which was embedded in sequences of situational action configurations. The notion of embedded practical knowledge raises questions about the real risk properties of this sport and it could be used to develop better educational programs for skydiving performance. These programs, based on series of situational action configurations, could also help novices to more quickly grasp one of the main keys to safety in risky sports: the well-known situational awareness defined as “the perception of the elements in the environment in a volume of time and space, the comprehension of their meaning and the projection of their status in the near future” (Endsley, |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|