The purpose of the study was to compare the self-paced and temporally constrained throwing performance in team-handball experts and novices without foreknowledge of target position. Our hypothesis was that throwing without foreknowledge of target position imposes a temporal constraint that would have a significant negative effect on the novices’ throwing performance, whereas, it would not significantly affect the experts’ performance. The main finding of our study is that there was a shot specific confirmation or rejection of our hypothesis, with an unexpected direction of the temporal constraint effect in the novices’ throwing accuracy. In specific, throwing velocity was significantly affected only in the 3Step Shot, not only in the novices but also in the experts, while, throwing accuracy was affected only in the Standing Shot, with a positive rather than negative effect in the novices. The training experience of the experts was comparable to that described for international level players, whereas our novices could be considered at a lower level (Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Laffaye et al., 2012; van den Tillaar & Ettema, 2006; Wagner et al., 2012). As expected, we found significantly greater ball-throwing velocity and better throwing accuracy in the experts than the novices with ball-throwing velocities within the range previously reported (Garcia et al., 2013; Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Gutiérrez-Dávila et al., 2013; Laffaye et al., 2012; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003, 2006; Wagner et al., 2011; Wagner et al., 2012). Also in agreement with previous studies, the ball-throwing velocity was greater in the 3Step Shot, followed by the Jump Shot and the Standing Shot; throwing accuracy did not differ among the three types of shots (Bayios et al., 2001; Wagner et al., 2011; 2012). We found a shot specificity in the change of throwing velocity and throwing accuracy which, most likely, indicates that the influence of temporal constraint may be determined by task specific biomechanical constraints. The presence or absence of ground contact together with the discrete differences of the lower body movements in the three types of shots are associated with different postural control demands. In particular, the Standing Shot involves keeping the lead foot on the floor, thus static postural control is more important throughout the throwing kinetic chain. In the 3Step Shot, one foot is planted on the floor after a 3 step run-up. Therefore, an effective transition from dynamic to static postural control is important for the efficient termination of the forward momentum and the provision of a stable support base to execute the throwing kinetic chain. Finally, the Jump Shot involves executing a single-leg vertical jump after the run-up with the throwing movement initiated and completed while the body is in the air. Such a situation demands dynamic postural control in the absence of ground contact. In throws such as the team-handball Standing Shot, research corroborates the existence of whole body postural control differences with respect to the temporal pressure (Ilmane and LaRue, 2008). Such a disturbance in head posture control and stabilization may be particularly important for aiming accuracy, because the control of head postural provides a fixed reference for the correct shoulder and body orientation for ball propulsion and release (Ripoll et al., 1986). The interaction between experience and temporal constraint condition also indicated a shot specificity of the temporal constraint’s influence, with similar effect in both the experts and the novices in the 3Step Shot but dissimilar effect in novices than experts in the Standing Shot. This dissimilarity may be associated to the less efficient postural and visual control of the novices. Previous studies report that visual (Jafarzadehpur et al. 2007; Vickers, 1996; Vickers et al., 2000; Williams et al., 2002) and postural control efficiency (Goonetilleke et al., 2009; Paillard and Noé, 2006; Paillard et al., 2011) distinguishes the expertise level and allows expert players less dependence on vision to control posture (Paillard and Noé, 2006; Paillard et al., 2011). Thus the task specific postural control demands in combination with the level of expertise may explain the shot specificity of the temporal constraint effect. The unexpected increase of throwing accuracy in the novices’ Standing Shot is not easy to explain. One possible explanation could be that depending on the task demands, novices may also benefit from limited execution time, as previously reported for experts (Beilock et al. 2004), however, this inference can not be adequately supported by our research design. This finding was unexpected because far-aiming accuracy is reported to worsen when the available execution time is reduced at 75% and 50% of the self-paced time in static tasks such as the billiard (Williams et al., 2002) and pistol shooting (Goonetilleke et al., 2009). In our study, the reduction of available execution time is reflected in the significant decrease of temporally constrained response times at about 50% to 70% of the self-paced condition. The decrease of far-aiming accuracy when under temporal pressure, is attributed to the reduction of the visual efficiency in information pick-up, typically recorded as a shorter “quiet eye” duration in billiard (Williams et al., 2002) and pistol shooting (Goonetilleke et al., 2009). Similar results are reported for dynamic far-aiming tasks encountered in soccer (Jordet et al., 2009) and basketball (de Oliveira et al., 2006; Oudejans et al., 2002). The “quiet eye” duration was not measured in our study and, to the best of our knowledge, there is no relevant research information in team-handball shots. Thus, the inclusion of visual control measures in future team-handball studies could elucidate such unexpected results of throwing performance. The testing conducted did not reflect the numerous situations faced by players during actual team-handball competition, which is a limitation also found in similar team-handball studies (Wagner et al., 2012). However, compared to previous studies, we believe our target electronic device (Bayios et al., 1998) increased the ecological setting of the experiment by enabling target characteristics that more closely replicate actual game shooting conditions. In previous handball studies, players were asked to throw at known, pre-determined locations that were continuously visible by players throughout various experimental conditions (Bourne et al., 2011; Garcia et al., 2013; Gutiérrez-Dávila et al., 2013; Wagner et al., 2012; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003; 2006). Such an experimental setting allows foreknowledge of the target position; this situation facilitates the endogenous preparation for the task even without external objects to process (Ruge et al., 2013; Sohn and Carlson, 2000). The increased ecological setting in our study is further enhanced due to the potential for 1946 “target hit pointers” anywhere within the 3.84 m2 θ -shaped tabloid surface (Bayios et al., 1998). This number of target positions far exceeds the number of targets previously used, including one (van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003, 2006; Wagner et al., 2012), four (Bourne et al., 2011; Gutiérrez-Dávila et al., 2013) and ten target positions (Garcia et al., 2013). The absence of a goalkeeper is a limitation of the study since its presence affects throwing velocity (Rivilla-Garcia et al., 2011a) and throwing accuracy (Rivilla-Garcia et al., 2011b) in a negative way with the effect being greater at lower expertise levels. However, this was also a limitation of many other studies on handball throwing (Garcia et al., 2013; Gutiérrez-Dávila et al., 2013; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003; 2006; Wagner et al., 2012). Furthermore, this limitation is not reflected in our results since the ball-throwing velocity was within the previously reported range for experts and novices (Garcia et al., 2013; Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Gutiérrez-Dávila et al., 2013; Laffaye et al., 2012; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003; 2006; Wagner et al., 2011; 2012). |