Research article - (2016)15, 501 - 508 |
Skills Associated with Line Breaks in Elite Rugby Union |
Steve den Hollander1, James Brown1,3, Michael Lambert1,3, Paul Treu4, Sharief Hendricks1,2, |
Key words: Rugby Union, performance, skill, technique, video analysis |
Key Points |
|
|
|
Procedure |
All matches (n = 125) of the 2013 Super Rugby competition were analysed. The 2013 Super Rugby competition was a professional men’s Rugby Union competition played between 15 franchises from Australia, New Zealand and South Africa. The footage was obtained from publically broadcasted matches that were recorded and saved onto a database. The study was ethically approved by the University of Cape Town Faculty of Health Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC REF: 559/2014). The recorded matches were analysed using Sports Code Elite Version 6.5.1 on an Apple iMac (Apple, USA) displayed at eye level. The analysis software allowed for the control of time lapse during the recorded match and the recording and saving of each event into a database. The analyst was able to pause, rewind and watch the footage in slow motion. The highest frequency at which the analyst could slow down the footage was 25 frames per second (25 Hz). Line breaks were identified using the following criteria. Note, all of the criteria described below had to be fulfilled for an event to be classified as a line break. The player in possession of the ball (i.e., ball-carrier) needed to visibly move through a straight line between two defenders in the defensive line or between a defender and the touchline, regardless of the shape and a defender and the touchline, regardless of the shape andmovement of the defensive line, and without being physically contacted by a defender (Gilmore, The defensive line needed to consist of at least two players moving in unison. After an attacker penetrated the defensive line, at least one defender from the defensive line was required to turn toward the ball-carrier, in an attempt to chase the ball-carrier. This criterion allowed for the distinction between half breaks and complete line breaks (Burt et al., The ball-carrier needed to receive the ball from a teammate before he breached the line. This criterion excluded interceptions and offloads from the analyses. When the starting phase was a turnover or loose ball, the line break could not occur in that phase of play, as it was deemed the defensive line would not have sufficient time to form. A total of 362 line breaks were identified, with an average of 3 (SD = 2) line breaks per match. Thereafter, each line break was coded for match, situational and technical descriptors that preceded the line break. The analysis of the line break began at the first set-piece, or change in possession, preceding the line break and ended if possession of the ball was lost, a break in open play after the line break occurred, when the ball went into touch, an infringement occurred or if a try was scored. |
Descriptors |
The purpose of the descriptors was to adequately describe the technical skills which led to a line break. The descriptors needed to possess objective metric properties and a valid means of interpretation (O’Donoghue, In line with the purpose of the study, the variables were divided into five categories: match situation, pre-line break characteristics, final phase characteristics, skills and post line break outcomes ( |
Reliability |
For intra-coder reliability, five randomly selected matches were coded twice using the variables and definitions described above. The coding of the same match was separated by a week (Wheeler et al., |
Statistical analysis |
The purpose of the statistical analyses was to determine the skills and match situations associated with line breaks. To achieve this, the first level of analyses was descriptive. The purpose of this was to describe and compare the frequency of occurrence. The second level of analyses tested the association between the skills and match situations variables that led to line-breaks using the Pearson’s Chi-squared test and Cramer’s V test. A Pearson’s Chi-squared P value of less than 0.05 was regarded as a significant association between the variables (Brace et al. The purpose of the final analyses was to determine the likelihood of skills and situational variables on post line break outcomes. Relative risk ratio (RRR), the ratio of the probability of an outcome occurring when a characteristic was observed (versus the non-observed characteristic) was determined using multinomial logistic regression. All statistics were done using STATA 11.1. |
|
|
The top five teams in the log, Thirty-nine percent of line breaks resulted in a The most common types of passes received by the line breaker were a |
Final phase characteristics |
The attacking line shape with the highest frequency was The majority of line breaks occurred when the ratio of attackers to defenders was an overlap of attackers, The distance of defence was significantly associated with the number of passes, type of pass, running speed, and running angle of the ball carrier. Passing the ball Line breaks occurred when the ball-carrier was running Faster defensive line speeds resulted in more line breaks [ The use of deception was found to be positively associated with the speed of defence [ |
Playing position |
Line breaks were achieved most frequently by |
Line breaks and tries |
The likelihood of a line break resulting in a |
|
|
This study showed that line breaks were associated with overall success in the Super Rugby competition and scoring tries. The technical attacking skills identified for line breaks were wide and deep positioning, receiving the ball at speed within 3 passes from the breakdown, and using passes that promote quick facilitation of the ball to outside backs. Furthermore, more line breaks were observed when these attacking technical skills were executed when the defensive line was further away and approaching quickly. In which case, the addition of a decoy or dummy runner proved useful to breach the defensive line untouched. Also, the skills associated with line breaks differed between playing positions. The top five teams in the competition (based on final log position) created the most line breaks, compared to the middle five or bottom five teams. In contrast, Wheeler et al. ( Ball-carrier speed as a key technical requirement for line breaks is not surprising. In an emerging environment like rugby, defenders (or a defender and the touchline) will inevitably close a wide gap in the defensive line to reduce opportunities to break the line. However, if the ball-carrier is able to identify the gap soon enough and accelerate and sprint through the gap faster than the defenders are able to close it, the ball-carrier will succeed in breaking the line (Correia et al. The closer the initial, final phase or line break field position was to the try line, the more likely the team was to lose possession of the ball in the phase following the line break. Diedrick and van Rooyen ( A limitation of the study was the absence of comparison phase outcome for line breaks. The lack of a comparator meant more sophisticated probability statistics could not be applied. Seemingly, tackle contact may be an appropriate comparator. However, all tackle contact events are not necessarily an unsuccessful line break, therefore tackle contact would have been invalid. Furthermore, the main purpose of this study was to describe non-contact skills that created line breaks, since effective contact have been described before (Wheeler & Sayers |
|
|
Line breaks are associated with overall team success and try scoring opportunities. Awareness of the defenders line speed and depth, fast running speed when receiving the ball and quick passing between attackers to the outside backs creates line break opportunities. During training, coaches should emphasise the movement speed of the ball between attackers and manipulate the speed and distance of the defenders. |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
This publication is based on research that has been supported in part by the University of Cape Town’s Research Committee (URC). The authors would like to thank Ryan Williams and Willie Maree for their assistance during the video analyses process. The authors would also like to thank the National Research Foundation Postdoctoral Innovation Research Fellowship. The methods used in this study comply with the current laws of the country in which they were performed. |
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY |
|
REFERENCES |
|