The purpose of this study was to investigate the reliability of short sprint performance and the reliability of the key kinematic determinants involved during the first three steps of the sprint. The aim was to use a practical method of kinematic analysis to help explain why changes may occur in sprint performance via the use of correlative statistics and also to provide reference values for intervention based research. It was hypothesised that short sprint performance would produce high reliability scores, with the kinematic variables demonstrating a lower level of reliability in comparison to performance. It was also hypothesised that step frequency and step length would show high levels of association with sprint performance, with variables such as stance time and flight time having a lesser level of association. The results of this study prove this hypothesis to be incorrect, with sprint performance not meeting the criteria to be considered a reliable measure. Despite this, many of the kinematic variables did prove to be adequately reliable and in some instances demonstrating high levels of reliability. Third step sprint determinants including step frequency and flight time returned small levels of association with sprint performance, with step length and stance time producing very small correlational relationships; therefore proving the secondary hypothesis to be partially correct. The reliability of sprint performance is a term used to describe the variance in sprint times over a number of trials or testing occasions (Hopkins, 2000). A review of the literature revealed it is common to see short sprint protocols (< 20m) fitting within the criteria required to be considered as either adequately reliable (ICC between 0.70 and 0.80, CV% < 10%), or in most cases highly reliable (ICC > 0.80, CV% < 10%) (Hopkins and Manly, 1989; Paungmali and Sitilertpisan, 2012; Shrout and Fleiss, 1979). Results from the current study were contrasting in comparison to past literature, as neither sprint distance (5m or 10m) met the criteria for either of these two categories. Despite having CV% < 10% (4.5% and 2.6%, respectively), the ICC values for the 5m (0.37) and 10m (0.62) distances did not reach the minimum reliability benchmark; therefore could not be considered as reliable measures. There are multiple reasons for why this discrepancy in results may have occurred between the current study and past literature. Firstly, the effort and intensity utilised by the subjects during each of their trials on the two separate testing days may not have been consistent. As previously stated, small changes (0.01s) in sprint times can be the difference between first and second place. When testing on different days, or over a number of trials, there will be a level of variation in times, even if the athletes sprint maximally every time (Hopkins, 2000). If a participant does not produce maximal efforts consistently throughout each trial / session, there is amplification of the variation and therefore a decrease in the level of reliability the data holds (Hopkins, 2000). The second possible cause of the discrepancies between this study and past literature is the methods and protocols used during testing. Factors such as timing light height, start protocol and start distance can all affect the overall reliability of sprint performance, although typically only by a small percentage (Cronin et al., 2007; Cronin and Templeton, 2008; Frost et al., 2008). Start distance (0.5m) and start protocol (preferred foot split stance) were kept consistent between sessions and the methods were derived from past literature, suggesting they should not have produced any discrepancies, assuming they remained constant throughout testing. It is possible that a combination of these factors united with the suggested lack of individual effort, may have contributed to the overall reliability scores. One trend that the data of this study did follow, was the lesser degree of variation over longer distances. The variation of the 5m distance (4.5%) was higher than at the 10m distance (2.6%), which imitates trends witnessed in several other studies reporting the reliability of sprint performance (Cronin et al., 2007; Moir et al., 2004). Cronin and Templeton (2008), suggest that the lower CV% at the longer distance is likely due to a larger amount of body variation at the shorter distances, with running becoming more consistent as the distance increases. Due to the fact the population assessed during the current study were team based athletes and not track sprinters, it is assumed that this theory can be applied in this instance also, as their starts are hypothesised to vary more between trials. As previously mentioned, sprint performance is largely dependent on a variety of kinetic and kinematic mechanical variables that ultimately produce movement (Bezodis et al., 2008; Morin et al., 2012). These mechanical variables can have a contributing effect on an individual’s sprint performance; therefore if they vary considerably between trials, it can be assumed that performance will display variance also. A review of the literature determined that there is very little data surrounding the reliability of the kinematic variables associated with sprinting. Hunter et al., (2004b), and Salo et al., (1996), investigated the reliability of many kinetic and kinematic variables associated with sprinting and sprint hurdles for individuals in late acceleration and running near top speed. These studies obtained reliability statistics for step length, step frequency, stance time, flight time and several body angles associated with touchdown and take-off, which revealed ICC values > 0.70, with many > 0.90 and all CV% values < 10%. Results of the current study somewhat mimic these findings; however not in every case. Step frequency, stance time, knee angle at touchdown and trunk angle at take-off revealed data which corresponded to either an adequate or high level of reliability during each of the first three steps. These values are similar to those witnessed in the studies by Hunter et al., (2004b), and Salo et al., (1996), which suggests in some instances, sprint mechanics are equally as constant nearing top speed as they are in the initial acceleration portion of a sprint. Contrasting to this statement, step length and knee angle at take-off revealed only one step out of the three to be reliable, with flight time not producing any. This could partially be explained by the findings of Salo et al., (1996), who found that step length was a measure that often varied between trials and required at least 11 trials to obtain a reliability score >0.70; therefore the three trials used in the current study may not have been enough to establish a constant average value. Another reason for the irregularities in these particular variables is simply due to the variance that occurs during the initial acceleration phase of individuals who do not train for consistency or strict form (such as our population sample), in comparison to someone such as a track sprinter (Cronin and Templeton, 2008). The advantage of these variables producing reliable data, is that this information can be used to help explain why any changes in performance may have occurred. If an intervention is implemented and an improvement in performance is seen, changes in the measured variables may help describe where and how this intervention has improved. For example, a post-activation potentiation intervention may improve sprint times by 0.2s, but due to the kinetics of performance being impractical to measure, an increase in step length (without a decrease in step frequency) would suggest a larger impulse on take-off. It must be noted that in order for this change to be considered as worthwhile, the change in step length must exceed the CV% achieved, which in the current study was 3.9% – 9%, depending on the step in question (Pearson et al., 2007). It can be stated that although these variables ranged in terms of consistency between trials, human movement over the first three steps does compare somewhat to movement later in a sprint when variability is concerned. The amount of influence these variables have on overall sprint performance can be identified via the investigation of the correlation between particular determinants of interest and sprint performance. Sprint performance is ultimately determined by a number of kinetic and kinematic variables, which interact with one another causing movement (Bezodis et al., 2008; Morin et al., 2012). Research has identified step length and step frequency as two of the key kinematic sprint determinants, as both have been reported to exhibit strong correlations with performance, although these values do fluctuate within literature (Hunter et al., 2004a; Salo et al., 2011). Stance time and flight time have also been identified as important characteristics in regards to short sprint performance, typically because of their influential relationship with step length and step frequency (Hunter et al., 2004b). The results of this study show dissimilar trends to these statements, as step frequency and stance time had the greatest level of association with 10m sprint performance. Although this was the case, these values (r = -0.386 and 0.396, respectively) only demonstrate a ‘small’ level of interaction between the variables and performance (Hopkins, 2002). These findings were accompanied by ‘very small’ levels of association for both step length and stance time, which again do not mimic typical sprinting theories. When comparing the scarce pool of literature surrounding this topic, it is evident that many of the results also vary from these generally accepted beliefs. For example, Hunter et al., (2004b), investigated the importance of step length and step frequency in relation to sprint velocity. The correlations found between sprint velocity and step length (r = 0.73) and step frequency (r = 0.14) were far different than those found by Morin et al., (2012), who reported data demonstrating step length (r = 0.363) had a much smaller influence than step frequency (0.897), when correlated with max treadmill speed. It is difficult to directly compare these results at face value with the current study, as it would have to be assumed that kinematic correlations with sprint performance will mimic the relationships witnessed for sprint velocity. The small pool of literature surrounding this topic leaves little opportunity for precise comparisons to be made, especially due to the varying nature of the testing procedures. Some research states track speed and treadmill speed are biomechanically different and cannot be generalised due to mechanical and kinetic irregularities between the two (Sinclair et al., 2013); however this conclusion is not consistent throughout literature and is currently still under investigation (Riley et al., 2008). This means conclusions about why these two previous studies may be different, or if values can be compared still remains unclear. The contradicting data-sets between the current study, Hunter et al., (2004b), and Morin et al., (2012), does however fit with the conflicting literature surrounding step length and step frequency, which suggests that although they are key sprint determinants, it is ultimately up to the physical characteristics of the subject to which variable plays a more defining role (Armstrong et al., 1984; Maulder et al., 2008; Mero et al., 1992; Murphy et al., 2003; Salo et al., 2011). It could therefore be possible that variations of this magnitude between three different studies could be explained by the preferences of the individual subjects taking part. The confidence intervals reported in this study suggest this assumption may have some feasibility, as ranges between r = -0.77 and r = 0.21 were identified, which demonstrate high levels of correlations for some subjects, with minimal associations with others. Another key factor that needs to be considered in the current study is the method of measuring kinematic variables. A study by Maćkała et al., (2015), reported a correlation of r = 0.83 between 30m step frequency and 30m sprint times. This method utilised a measurement style which encompassed all the steps over a 30m distance, whereas studies such as Hunter et al., (2004b), used only a single step 16m from the starting point and in this current study measures were taken on the third ground contact. These irregularities in measuring strategies may help explain some of the differences between the studies; however due to the small amount of literature surrounding this topic, it is difficult to state which method is preferred. Stance times and flight times were investigated by Morin et al., (2012), who found correlations of -0.852 (p < 0.01) and -0.018 (p = 0.95) with maximal treadmill running speed and -0.751 (p< 0.01) and 0.773 (p = 0.88), respectively for 100m performance. This again proves much different to the values obtained for stance time (-0.088) and flight time (0.396) in the present study, which suggests that the treadmill stride patterns may differ than those witnessed during a linear sprint method; therefore backing the conclusions of Sinclair et al., (2013). The findings of Morin et al., (2012), do however compare well with the results of Murphy et al., (2003), who did not perform a correlative study with performance, but identified that there was a significant (p = 0.01) difference between the stance times of ‘slow’ and ‘fast’ athletes. This interaction of results suggests that stance times may in-fact have a greater influence on sprint times than witnessed in the values derived from the current study. Confidence intervals of this study suggest that in some individuals, there was higher correlations of flight and stance times than the final value suggests; therefore it is concluded that kinematic determinants do vary between runners and this concept needs to be recognised when working in group environments. Measurements of stance times and flight times over a number of steps is recommended in order to find which step, or combination of step values (if any), holds the strongest correlation with sprint performance, as currently data pool is scare and still unclear. One limitation of this study is the number of participants recruited to assist with this investigation. It was concluded by Salo et al., (1996), that the larger the pool of participants utilised during a reliability study, the stronger the results become, as outliers and irregular trials have a lesser influence on the overall outcome. Although ten participants returned a substantial amount of data in the current study, it can be assumed a larger pool of participants would have retuned more reliable results. A second limitation within this study is the number of trials used to establish reliability. Hunter et al., (2004b), found that reliability figures improved when averages of numerous trials were used, with the variance decreasing as trial numbers increased. This trend is consistent with the findings of Salo et al., (1996), who identified that kinematic variables required anywhere from 1-35 trials in order to reach a reliability rating of >0.70. This means that in the current study, a larger number of trials per participant may have provided a more accurate mean value and therefore more reliable results than those achieved by averaging just three trials. The two sessions involved the setup of high speed cameras, timing lights and also body markers on the participating individuals. In order to maintain as little variability as possible, all timing light and camera markers were left marked between sessions, with precautionary re-measurements occurring prior to the second session beginning. Joint markers were kept as consistent as possible by using the same experienced researcher for all the subjects in both sessions; however, there is a possibility that small variations in these markings could have influenced the data analysis process. |