Thirteen NCAA Division I, male, tennis players (Age = 20 ± 3 years; Height = 1.85 ± 0.06 m; Mass = 76 ± 9 kg; 10 were right-hand dominant) volunteered to participate in this study. Each participant completed one data collection that lasted approximately two hours. Participants arrived at a university biomechanics lab, received instructions describing data collection procedures, and then provided informed consent. Data collection procedures were approved by the appropriate institutional review board prior to data collection. Each participant used his own tennis racquet throughout data collection. In the biomechanics lab, eight calibrated high-speed video cameras (480 Hz; VICON, Centennial, CO, USA; Figure 2) were used to track the motion of a marked tennis racquet (Figure 3). The calibrated motion capture volume was approximately 3 m3 and centered near the location of the expected ball-racquet impact. Although we did not quantify accuracy of the reconstructed spatial coordinates, we followed manufacturer guidelines in calibrating each motion capture volume. Previously, using a comparable calibration volume size and identical calibration procedures, we calculated the root mean square error of the coordinate reconstruction to be approximately 0.1 mm. We measured ball TAV in the lab using a different high-speed video camera (Frame Rate = 240 Hz; Exposure Time = 0.002 s; Resolution = 1920 x 1080 pixels; NEX-FS700U, SONY, Japan; Figure 2). The optical axis of the Sony camera was directed toward the expected position of ball-racquet contact and aligned nearly parallel to the expected direction of the forehand groundstrokes (Figure 2). The tennis balls were marked with two black orthogonal lines to facilitate the measurement of ball TAV. We quantified the spatial position of the marked racquet (Figure 3) to measure hitting zone length, racquet head impact angle, racquet trajectory before impact, and racquet horizontal and vertical velocity before impact. To familiarize participants with the forehand topspin task (i.e., hitting topspin forehand groundstrokes, in the laboratory, using a marked racquet), participants hit approximately 10 forehand groundstrokes toward a target (a 1-m diameter hoop) that was 9.5 m from the subject and approximately 1.75 m high (Figure 2). A ball machine (Tennis Tutor Prolite, Sport Tutor Inc, Burbank, CA, USA) was positioned behind the target (Figure 2) and fed tennis balls to the participants at a speed of 11 m/s. After leaving the ball machine, the fed balls bounced once on the lab floor, to a height that was approximately shoulder high, and were struck by the participants. Participants were instructed to hit the forehands, at the target, at the same intensity as their best topspin forehand groundstroke, during a competitive tennis match. After the 10 familiarization forehands, participants were instructed to hit their best topspin forehand groundstroke, toward the target. Participants performed ten successful forehand groundstrokes, with 20 s between each stroke. Strokes were deemed successful when (1) the participant successfully hit the target area and (2) researcher and participant agreed that the stroke was representative of a high-quality, competitive, forehand groundstroke. A small number of trials were rejected because they did not meet the aforementioned criteria. Although we did not record the number of rejected forehand groundstrokes, we estimate that approximately 2 trials, on average, were rejected, per subject. Following these lab trials, participants went to a nearby indoor tennis court to perform the forehand accuracy test. On this court, a tennis ball machine was positioned just behind the center mark on the opposite baseline and balls were fed, at 12.5 m/s (landing 0.5 m behind the service line), to the participants every 4 s. Participants hit 50 topspin forehand groundstrokes (25 cross-court and 25 down-the-line) toward a target area that was made up of three different sub-targets (Figure 4). During these on-court trials, we instructed the participants in an identical manner, relative to the laboratory trials: i.e., the participants were instructed to hit the forehands, at the target, at the same intensity they would use to generate their best topspin forehand groundstroke, in a competitive setting. Each shot was scored depending on which sub-target area the ball landed in (Target 1 =1 point; Target 2 = 2 points; Target 3 = 3 points). Earned points were totaled for all 50 forehand strokes, as well as for the 25 cross-court and 25 down-the-line strokes. As each groundstroke was performed, the same researcher visually scored each shot and totaled all scores. Forehand accuracy was quantified as the earned score, divided by the total possible score, multiplied by one hundred. This method of evaluating forehand groundstroke accuracy is similar to a previously described approach, used to evaluate serving accuracy (Fernandez-Fernandez et al., 2014). We chose to incorporate two shot directions to evaluate forehand accuracy because we believed this would more comprehensively reflect each subject’s ability to accurately hit a forehand groundstroke in a competitive setting. We quantified five explanatory variables: hitting zone length, racquet head impact angle, racquet trajectory before impact (relative to horizontal), and racquet horizontal and vertical velocity before impact. We quantified hitting zone length as the distance, in the horizontal plane, the racquet center (midpoint between Markers 2 and 5; Figure 3) traveled while the racket was between two orientations: 5Ëš less than the orientation at impact (before impact) and 5Ëš greater than the orientation at impact (after impact). In other words, we measured how far the racquet center exhibited quasi-translation, immediately before and after impact (Gensemer, 1985); we chose this 10Ëš range of motion, because we believed it somewhat represented the previously-described hitting zone (Chafin and Moore, 1994; Gensemer, 1985; 1994). Racquet head impact angle was quantified as the angle depicted in Figure 1A, near the time of impact (Choppin et al., 2011). More specifically, the orientation between two lines: a line connecting Reflective Markers 2 and 5 (VICON; Figure 3), and a fixed horizontal axis (Figure 1A). Although the location of ball impact (on the racquet face), relative to the long axis of on the racquet, varies, this variation was expected to be random and not systematically influence our measured impact angles. Racquet trajectory, relative to horizontal, before impact was quantified as the mean orientation of the angle depicted in Figure 2B (angle from the horizontal), between 42 ms prior to impact and impact (this was an arbitrarily chosen duration that allowed for averaging of data on impact, as the racquet wobbles upon impact with the ball). For the variables of hitting zone length, racquet head impact angle, racquet trajectory before impact (relative to horizontal), and racquet horizontal and vertical velocity before impact, 10 discrete values were collected for each participant, representing the 10 lab trials. Unfiltered data were used in calculating the explanatory variables, to avoid any smoothing of true signal, and we acknowledge potential risk for error associated with this approach (Knudson and Bahamonde, 2001; Tanabe and Ito, 2007). Because we measured the position data with such a high sampling rate (480 Hz), however, and the position data were expected to be quite accurate (RMS ~ 0.1 mm), we supposed that the analysis of raw data would suffice; to test this idea, we tested all trials for three of our subjects, by filtering the coordinate data using a 4th order, recursive, low-pass, Butterworth filter (17 Hz cutoff frequency). We compared three methods: (1) unfiltered, (2) filtering the entire trial, through impact, and (3) the linear extrapolation technique described by Knudson and Bahamonde (2001). For each of three subjects, the three aforementioned approaches resulted in nearly identical results, for three different tested explanatory variables: racquet trajectory before impact (relative to horizontal), and racquet horizontal and vertical velocities before impact. Results of these analyses are presented in Appendix A. We quantified two response variables: ball TAV and forehand accuracy. We measured ball TAV using the aforementioned high-speed Sony camera (this was the only variable measured using this camera). We counted the number of video frames that elapsed during three complete ball rotations, immediately following racquet-ball impact. This number of frames was then divided by the frame rate (240 frames/s), to calculate time. The number of rotations (three) was then divided by time, which equaled the mean ball TAV (rotations per second). For ball TAV, 10 discrete values were collected and averaged for each participant (representing the 10 lab trials). Forehand accuracy was quantified as one value for cross-court strokes, one value for down-the-line strokes and one value for all of the strokes (total accuracy); these measures of accuracy were the only variables calculated on an actual tennis court. Means and standard deviations for all of the explanatory and response variables were calculated. We used mixed model regression, with blocking on subject, to evaluate potential relationships between (1) the racquet kinematics (hitting zone length, racquet head impact angle, racquet vertical trajectory (relative to horizontal) before impact, and racquet horizontal and vertical velocity before impact) and (2) ball TAV. We used a multiple linear regression approach to evaluate potential relationships between the racquet kinematics and forehand groundstroke accuracy: cross-court, down-the-line, and total accuracy. For both the mixed model and multiple regression analyses, depending on the resulting p values, we used a forward-selection, stepwise, regression approach to construct a statistical model that best explained variation in the response variables. Scatterplots were used to evaluate the linearity assumptions for the relationships between predictor and response variables. An alpha level of 0.05 was set initially, however, to control the family-wise error rate, we adjusted each p value using Holm’s step down method (Holm, 1979). We calculated coefficients of variation for each variable measured in the lab, to determine trial to trial variance for these measures, and these results are presented in Appendix B. |