Results of the included studies were summarized in Table 3. Eight studies investigated the relationship between match period and RDM (Ahmed et al., 2017; Catteeuw et al., 2010; Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; Larkin et al., 2014; Mallo et al., 2012; Mascarenhas et al., 2009), followed by six studies that examined the relationship between referees’ velocity and RDM (EL; Catteeuw et al., 2010; Elsworthy et al., 2014a; Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; Mascarenhas et al., 2009; Oudejans et al., 2005). Three studies investigated the relationship between the referees’ heart rate and RDM (Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; Mascarenhas et al., 2009) and two studies assessed the relationship between the total distance covered and RDM (Emmonds et al., 2015; Mascarenhas et al., 2009). Finally, one study examined the relationship between blood lactate (Larkin et al., 2014) respectively running time and RDM (Paradis et al., 2015). Match period. Five studies found a negative relationship between match period and RDM, which means that with increasing playing time RDM decreased (Ahmed et al., 2017; Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; Mallo et al., 2012; Samuel et al., 2019). Another five studies did not find evidence of a negative or positive relationship between match period and RDM (Ahmed et al., 2017; Catteeuw et al., 2010; Emmonds et al., 2015; Mascarenhas et al., 2009; Samuel et al., 2019). Two studies revealed an increase of RDM with increasing playing time (Emmonds et al., 2015; Larkin et al., 2014). Samuel et al. (2019; soccer) found a drop in RDM from the second to the third quarter (effect size: d = 0.91) as well as from the third to the fourth quarter (d = 0.66). Two studies, carried out in futsal (Ahmed et al., 2017) and soccer (Mallo et al., 2012), showed that RDM dropped in the second half of a game. In contrast, Samuel et al. (2019) did not find a difference between half times. Likewise, Ahmed et al. (2017) did not find a drop in RDM at contentious decisions either. Three studies did not reveal a relationship between 10-minute or 15-minute periods and RDM (soccer: Catteeuw et al., 2010; rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; soccer: Mascarenhas et al., 2009). However, two studies found a decrease in RDM in the last 10 minutes (rugby: d = 0.86, Emmonds et al., 2015) respectively 15 minutes (soccer: Mallo et al., 2012) of a match. At last, two studies revealed a positive relationship between playing time and RDM (rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; Australian football: Larkin et al., 2014). Velocity. The Relationship between referees’ velocity and RDM was analysed with the instantaneous referees’ velocity at the time of the decision (Catteeuw et al., 2010; Elsworthy et al., 2014a; Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; soccer: d = 0.02, Mascarenhas et al., 2009; Oudejans et al., 2005) and with the running speed prior to the decision (Elsworthy et al., 2014a). Examining first the relationship between the referees’ instantaneous velocity and RDM, two studies revealed a decrease of RDM with increasing velocity (soccer: Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; soccer: Oudejans et al., 2005) while four studies did not indicate a positive or negative relationship between the referees’ velocity and RDM (soccer: Catteeuw et al., 2010; Australian football: Elsworthy et al., 2014a; rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; soccer: d = 0.02, Mascarenhas et al., 2009). A closer look at the two studies that reveal a negative relationship between the referees’ velocity and RDM indicates contradictory findings: Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega (2016) found that most errors occur at slow velocity (<8 km/h; soccer), while Oudejans et al. (2005) found that errors occur more often at higher velocities (> 8km/h; soccer). As mentioned, four studies show no change of RDM in terms of the distribution of errors according to six different categories of speed (standing: 0.0 – 0.1 m/s, walking: 0.2 – 1.9 m/s, jogging: 2.0 – 3.9 m/s, running: 4.0 – 5.4 m/s, high-intensity running: 5.5 – 6.9 m/s, sprinting: >7.0 m/s; soccer: Catteeuw et al., 2010) nor to the instantaneous velocity at the time of the decision (Australian football: Elsworthy et al., 2014a; rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; soccer: Mascarenhas et al., 2009). At least, Elsworthy et al. (2014a) investigated the relationship between RDM and the referees’ velocity five seconds, thirty seconds, one minute and five minutes prior to the decision. Results indicate that the referees’ velocity five seconds prior to incorrect decisions was significantly higher compared to correct decisions. Running time. Paradis et al. (2015) investigated the relationship between running time and RDM in Australian football (Australian football: Paradis et al., 2015), but they did neither find a negative nor positive relationship. Distance covered. No relationship was found between the total distance covered and RDM (rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; soccer: d = 0.27, Mascarenhas et al., 2009). Physiological parameters. Two studies did not find a relationship between heart rate and RDM (rugby: Emmonds et al., 2015; soccer: d = 0.01, Mascarenhas et al., 2009), while one study revealed a drop in RDM especially when the heart rate was above 85% (soccer: Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016). One study examined physical exertion with blood lactate did not find evidence for a relationship between physical exertion and RDM (Australian football: Larkin et al., 2014). Overall, most findings in this review did not reveal a negative or positive relationship between physical load and RDM (n = 18; Ahmed et al., 2017; Catteeuw et al., 2010; Elsworthy et al., 2014a; Emmonds et al., 2015; Larkin et al., 2014; Mascarenhas et al., 2009; Paradis et al., 2015; Samuel et al., 2019). However, n = 13 findings point to a negative relationship between physical load and RDM (Ahmed et al., 2017; Elsworthy et al., 2014a; Emmonds et al., 2015; Gomez-Carmona and Pino-Ortega, 2016; Mallo et al., 2012; Oudejans et al., 2005; Samuel et al., 2019), while only two studies suggest an increase of RDM with increasing load (match period; Emmonds et al., 2015; Larkin et al., 2014). |