In this study, we sought to identify the optimal approach to enhance psycho-physiological recovery after a swimming race in national level youth swimmers. To achieve this, we compared the effects of two after-competition protocols on BL, HR, and RPE levels. Secondly, we also investigated whether higher-ranked swimmers would also present higher rates of recovery than the rest of their competitors. We found that the experimental recovery protocol appears more efficient than the coach-prescribed one in improving BL. However, there were no differences between groups regarding HR and RPE levels measured post-recovery. Moreover, we observed no influence of attained ranking position in BL, HR or RPE rates of recovery. Predictably, we found significant differences between post-competition and post-recovery BL, HR, and RPE values, both within the experimental and coach-prescribed groups. Moreover, the between groups analysis casted further doubts upon the employment of HR and RPE as reliable means to quantify the best recovery protocol after a swimming race. That is, no differences were found in HR and RPE between the experimental and coach-prescribed groups at the post-recovery phase. In fact, although post exercise HR can adequately represent cardiovascular fatigue for a single group, the individual variation is too wide for this to be a useful measurement while being subject to external stimuli and comparing for different interventions (Bassey, 1996). Furthermore, despite the RPE being indicated as an ecological and valid tool to assess and quantify loads in swimming (Wallace et al., 2009), some issues when comparing interventions could reside in the fact that it remains a subjective scale, and that asking to quantify fatigue from 0 to 10 could “flatten out” the outcomes of an inquiry conducted with high level athletes competing at the same level, as it happened in the present study. Nonetheless, BL was the only parameter which returned significant differences between the experimental and coach-prescribed group at the post-recovery phase. In particular, the experimental group presented significant lower levels of BL compared to the coach-prescribed group. Hence, we do not recommend employing exclusively HR and RPE when comparing short-term interventions to enhance recovery in high level swimmers, since the possible difference between groups appears to be excessively subtle for these two kinds of measures. Instead, in line with previous investigations, we suggest using HR (Koenig et al., 2014; Ganzevles et al., 2017) and RPE (Wallace et al., 2009; Czelusniak et al., 2021) as tools to repeatedly test the single group/swimmer over a longer time span, being useful to assess cardiovascular as well as psychological progresses across the whole competitive season. As noted earlier, interesting results came from the BL levels analysis, i.e., the experimental protocol made the swimmers recover more quickly than the coach-prescribed protocol. In consideration of our outcomes, we then suggest scholars and coaches interested in enhancing swimmers’ anaerobic recovery to follow the conceptual foundation of the experimental protocol, i.e., designing in-water, active recovery protocols which range from an easy to a moderate pace, vary rest times, exercises, and materials used, all while not shying away from requesting the swimmers to go at their hardest pace, perhaps for short distances. Our recommendation, however, is in contrast with Lomax (Lomax, 2012), which found that after intense swimming, a recovery protocol consisting of self-paced, continuous steady rate swimming is equally effective in lowering BL levels than a swimming recovery consisting of various strokes, intensities, and rest intervals. The protocols used by these investigators was similar to the coach-prescribed protocol employed in this study. However, it is worth noting that the colleagues recruited regional level swimmers (whereas we recruited national level youth swimmers), also employing the 200m race-paced front crawl in a controlled environment to elicit fatigue. Moreover, there is a compelling element of diversity (i.e., swimming race vs. controlled environment) that well highlights the particularity of our experiment compared to other investigations in the topic (Toubekis et al., 2008; Ali Rasooli et al., 2012; Pratama and Yimlamai, 2020). Mainly, we based on previous works which suggested that swimming performance measurements could have important implications within a competition setting (Faghy et al., 2019; Shell et al., 2020). Reasonably, the competitive environment already stimulated and motivated the swimmers to perform at their highest level, thus producing “actual” maximum levels of overall fatigue. It would be of interest to test whether our results could be replicated in a more controlled and less stressful environment, or if the competition setting is instead necessary for specific protocols to significantly arouse enhanced recovery from exercise-related fatigue. Some parameters of physical conditioning were shown to be able to predict swimming performance among youth swimmers, i.e., maximum force-velocity exertion (Sorgente et al., 2023) and mechanics (Pérez-Olea et al., 2018) in the pull-up motion, as well as overall upper (Lopes et al., 2021) and lower limb (Crowley et al., 2018) maximum strength capacities. Considering the closed-skill nature swimming, it is thus widely recognized that the most performing swimmers normally correspond to the most physically accomplished (Bravi et al., 2022). However, recovery capacities do not appear to follow the same trend. Specifically, the MANOVA we implemented resulted in no significant interactions of recovery protocols on indicators of fatigue when controlling for race ranking obtained. Thus, we cannot consider BL, HR, RPE, as valuable indicators nor predictors for swimming performance when comparing different recovery protocols. Notwithstanding, this could also mean that the recovery protocols here employed equally contributed to enhance recovery parameters regardless of the differences in achieved ranking by each swimmer. However, more focused, carefully designed studies should be conducted to confirm this assertion, e.g., by assessing multiple, heterogeneous groups of swimmers competing at different levels. Surely, some elements from our experimental design could be ameliorated. For instance, we gathered the data at a specific time of the competitive season, which should supposedly be at the peak form of each athlete. However, we did not control any aspect of training in preparation to the competition. Therefore, dedicated research should focus on collecting the fluctuations and changes of physiological indicators of fatigue, especially BL, before and after swimming race/recovery protocols throughout the season. Furthermore, it is worth noting that we opted for a higher (and unexplored) ecological value for this study, i.e., measuring BL, HR, and RPE during a national-level swimming event. This also meant operating at high temporal efficiencies due to the tight schedule of the competitions. For these reasons, we chose to only collect one BL measurement, and a 5-seconds average of the HR, per subject (for each phase). These kind of procedures for BL and HR assessment have been already used and standardized by other scholars (Tanner et al., 2010; Schubert et al., 2018); hence, they can be indeed telling of the physiological amount of fatigue elicited in swimmers after maxing out their performance. On the other hand, seeing that the lactate peak could subjectively occur at different times after maximal/supramaximal efforts, taking only one measurement may not be as accurate as considering, for instance, the lactate kinetic, which in turn would be more time-consuming. Consequently, the same could be stated concerning the employment of various HR measurements, where maximal HR during a specific time period, the R-R interval, or the HR kinetic could be used or combined in lieu of our approach. However, further research is required to test whether different kinds of BL and HR detection could bring significantly different result from one to another, in the ecological context of high-level swimming races. In other words, the present experimental approach regarding the strategies of BL and HR data collection holds some limitations. One is that, by standardizing the timing of our data collection, we ruled out paramount inter-subjective differences. The other one is that, by offering only a “snapshot” about the swimmers’ physiological levels of stress after competition and their respective recovery after the protocols here proposed, more accurate (thus more time-consuming) assessments of these parameters could bring different results than the ones we found with this experimental design. Because of these factors, it is important to state that there is an inherent (although systemic) limitation in our study, and that the interpretation of our results should be approached with caution. Nevertheless, a recent work conducted by Mavroudi et al., (2023) found the lactate peak after different all-out swimming sprints (25m, 35m, and 50m) occurring after 2 minutes from the trial. This is rather compelling, given that the lactate peak is usually thought to occur between 4 and 10 minutes after a maximal physical effort. While only partially confirming our approach, this suggests that systematizing and standardizing the timing of BL collection could be practical in detecting common trends in BL behavior after intense bout of swimming, as well as the BL response to a certain type of recovery protocol from these exertions, in spite of the individual differences between athletes. Another critical element could pertain to the duration of the recovery protocols here employed, i.e., 20 minutes circa, which was fixed regardless of the distance specialization. Surely, it would be expected to design dedicated recovery protocols for sprinters and for middle-distance swimmers, given that the shorter the event is, the longer a swimmer should cool down (Riewald, 2015). This is because the intensity of the swimming bout determines how high blood lactate concentration will rise (Neric et al., 2009). For these reasons, sprinters can produce higher lactate concentrations than endurance athletes do, needing more time to clear lactate from their bodies (Issurin, 2010). However, we argue that there is an antecedent pitfall in swimming performance science. That is, usual recovery times employed for high-level swimmers do not continuously last more than 5-10 minutes (Toubekis et al., 2008). Other scholars pointed out that more time would be advisable. For instance, Riewald suggested that a general proper recovery protocol should consist of at least 15 minutes of active swimming, based on the fact that the lactate concentration rises over the first several minutes after the race, then, over the next 20 to 30 minutes, this concentration declines to near baseline or pre-race levels (Riewald, 2015). The decrement in fatigue between these 20 to 30 minutes happens for any kind of swimmer, regardless if they are sprinters or middle-distance specialists. Hence, leveraging on the latter notion, we opted for a generalized, systematic, and “no-rush” approach for all the swimmers involved, regardless of the distance specialization. Furthermore, despite the differences about lactate characteristics in distance and gender documented in Holfelder et al. (2013), our results highlighted the importance of a recovery protocol employing a relatively large volume (around 1000 meters) and different intensities for a suitable and generally feasible fatigue dissipation, without considering the event or gender in which the swimmer has competed. In such wise, given that BF, HR and RPE levels did not differ within our two respective groups, this study revealed no differences in recovery capacities between sprinters and middle-distance swimmers, as well as among the four strokes. This would be beneficial for the generalizability aspect of these recovery protocols, which could lead to an even more efficient degree of control and comparison of the swimmers’ specialization profiles, regarding both the specific stroke and distance expertise. It remains to be seen, however, whether our recommended experimental protocol could be time-optimized for endurance swimmers as well, such as open water swimmers or long-distance ones (i.e., swimmers specialized in 800m and 1500m races). Specific age-category or gender differences should be taken into account when discussing the implementation of optimal after-competition recovery protocols. Regarding age, it is worth stressing that for the present study we only recruited national level swimmers competing at the junior level, i.e., the mean age was of 15 ± 1.1 years. Research in the topic of swimming recovery has focused on either elite (Vescovi et al., 2011; Ali Rasooli et al., 2012; Faghy et al., 2019), master (Reaburn and Mackinnon, 1990), collegiate (Pratama and Yimlamai, 2020) or regional level swimmers (Lomax, 2012; Sorgente et al., 2023), with many others referring to competitive/well-trained swimmers without further specifications (Wakayoshi et al., 1992; Buchheit et al., 2010; Kabasakalis et al., 2020). From a competitive standpoint, however, national level youth swimmers represent the most promising category for leveling up at the elite of the sport (Mitchell et al., 2021), making this specific age-level population worth of dedicated investigation. Nevertheless, we did not make any comparison between age-categories, e.g., senior vs. junior, as it was not the purpose of our investigation. Thus, the results from this study should be read under a specific filter of age and competitive level. It remains to be seen whether and how the approach used in this study would be effective for elder categories of swimmers, also considering the different training demands that comes with further progression in the sporting career. Concerning the gender comparison, possible differences between males and females swimming performances have been explained due to physiological, psychological, anthropometrical and biomechanical aspects (Knechtle et al., 2020). In contrast with this review, however, we did not find differences in BL nor HR or RPE between male and female competitors. To this regard, it is worth noting that Rascon et al. reported that RPE and BL did not differ between genders in determining exercise intensity response, while females had higher HR than males (Rascon et al., 2020). Thus, our results are partially in line with previous research which stated that BL and RPE are gender-independent markers of physical exertion (Korhonen et al., 2005). Thus, the approach here used towards immediate recovery post-competition appears to function regardless of the gender. However, the extent and robustness of this finding should be better investigated with dedicated research about differences in recovery rates between male and female swimmers. |