Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
ISSN: 1303 - 2968   
Ios-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Views
9272
Download
1281
from September 2014
 
©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2011) 10, 534 - 539

Research article
Influence Of Opposition On Ball Velocity In The Handball Jump Throw
Jesús Rivilla-Garcia1, , Ignacio Grande1, Javier Sampedro1, Roland Van Den Tillaar2,3  
Author Information
1 Faculty of Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Spain
2 Health and Human Development, Vila Real, Portugal
3 Department of Teacher Education and Sports of Sogn and Fjordane University College, Norway

Jesús Rivilla-Garcia
✉ Department of Sports, Faculty of Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid, Spain
Email: esus.rivilla@upm.es
Publish Date
Received: 18-03-2011
Accepted: 15-07-2011
Published (online): 01-09-2011
 
ABSTRACT

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the influence of different degrees of opposition on ball velocity in the jump throw in elite, amateur and adolescent team handball players. Thus, one hundred and nineteen elite, amateur and under 18 team handball players performed jump throws under three different conditions: 1) without opposition, 2) with the opposition of the goalkeeper and 3) with the opposition of the goalkeeper and a defensive player. The degree of opposition was found to have a negative effect on ball velocity in all three groups (p < 0.001). Furthermore, the level of competition had a positive effect on ball velocity (p < 0.001). However, no interaction was found between the level of competition and the degree of opposition on ball velocity (p = 0.178). The findings of this study indicate that an increase of external stimuli probably influences throwing kinematics and thereby maximal ball velocity. However, experience does not seem to be a factor that can reduce the influence of these external stimuli.

Key words: Team handball, jump throw, ball velocity, defence


           Key Points
  • The degree of opposition had a negative effect upon ball throwing velocity in elite, amateur and adolescent handball players in the jump throw.
  • It indicated that an increase of external stimuli influences the execution of throwing.
  • Experience does not seem to be a factor that can reduce the influence of these external stimuli.

INTRODUCTION

TTeam handball is an Olympic team sport in which two teams of seven players each (six outfield players and a goalkeeper) try to score points by throwing a handball into the goal of the other team. The team that scores most goals after two periods wins. To achieve this, team handball players require high levels of physical skills that include jumping, diving, blocking, sprinting, ball control and agility. One of the most important skills for success in team handball is throwing ability (Gorostiaga et al., 2005). Perhaps for this reason many studies have analyzed throwing technique (Fradet et al., 2004; Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Granados et al., 2007; Jöris et al., 1985; Pori et al., 2005; Sibila et al., 2003; Van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2004; 2007; Wagner et al., 2008).

The success of throwing is influenced by its accuracy (Bayios and Boudoulos, 1998; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003a; 2004; 2007) and ball velocity (Bayios et al., 2001; Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Marques et al., 2007; Sibila et al., 2003, Wagner et al., 2010a).

Several studies have indicated that ball velocity was determined by throwing technique, the timing of the consecutive actions of body segments, and upper and lower-extremity muscle strength and power (Jöris et al., 1985; Van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2007; Van Muijen et al., 1991). However, team handball players use different throwing techniques when defensive players are involved (Wagner et al., 2010a) and select the direction of the ball according to the movements of the goalkeeper.

In team handball the most used throw at the goal is the jump throw made from 9 m from the goal (Wagner et al., 2008). An opponent is often between the thrower and the goal, which could influence the kinematics and the throwing velocity of the attacker. Furthermore, the goalkeeper has to be surprised by the throw. Therefore, different degrees of opposition could influence throwing velocity. However, most studies are performed without any opposition (e.g. Fradet et al., 2004; Wagner et al., 2008; van den Tillaar and Ettema, 2003a; 2007), which could influence maximal throwing velocity. Only Gutiérrez et al., 2006 studied the influence of opposition on the team handball jump throw. They found no differences in maximal ball velocity between jump throws with and without opposition in experienced team handball players. These findings indicate that when accuracy was more important (to overcome a defence player and a goalkeeper) the throwing execution was not influenced and thereby escaped the velocity- accuracy trade-off. This velocity-accuracy trade-off suggests that when focusing on accuracy velocity would decrease (Fitts, 1954). According to van den Tillaar and Ettema (2003b) an explanation for this finding could lie in the specific subject group. The subjects in the study by Gutiérrez et al., 2006 were highly experienced team handball players who were not influenced by opposition in their execution of throwing during a jump throw. However, is this also the case when less experienced players perform the same jump throw or when the degree of opposition is varied?

Therefore the aim of this study was to investigate the influence of different degrees of opposition on ball velocity in the jump throw in elite, amateur and adolescent team handball players. It was hypothesized that the increase in the degree of opposition would result in a decrease of maximal throwing velocity since accuracy in throwing becomes more important when a player has to overcome a defence player and a goalkeeper compared to only surprising a goalkeeper. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that high level players would decrease throwing velocity less when the degree of opposition increased compared to less experienced players, since elite players have more knowledge about the different situations than novice players. Experts not only know what to do in a wide variety of situations, but they also know how and when to apply this knowledge and they are able to reproduce it in the appropriate situations (Singer and Janelle, 1999).

METHODS

Participants

One hundred and nineteen team handball players from nine handball teams who were classified into three groups according to their competitive level: elite, amateur and under 18, participated in this study. The elite group consisted of two teams who played in the highest Spanish team handball division. The amateur group also consisted of two teams from the senior category (over 18) who played in the second Spanish team handball division. While the under 18 group was made up of three teams still in their training phase (of under 18 years of age) who played in the regional youth division. The characteristics of each group are presented in Table 1. Before performing the tests, all the players, and the parents or guardians in the case of the participants under 18, were fully informed about the procedure to be followed as well as the potential risks, and signed a consent form to participate in the study. The procedure was in accordance with the approval of the local ethics committee and followed current ethical standards in sports and exercise research.

Procedure

The maximal ball velocity of the participants was evaluated performing the jump throw in three different situations: a) without any opposition: shooting at goal without the presence of the goalkeeper or a defensive player; b) with the opposition of only the goalkeeper and c) with the opposition of the goalkeeper and a defensive player.

The test was conducted after at least 24 hours of active rest and at least 48 hours after a competition. After a general warm-up, which included running, low intensity exercises and flexibility, a standard 10 minute specific warm-up was performed consisting of various specific moves, and acceleration and deceleration runs similar to the steps taken prior to a throw. This was followed by specific mobility exercises for the shoulder and throws with balls of different mass and varied jumps of progressive specificity and intensity. The specific warm-up ended with actions similar to the tests. The participants were then informed about the protocol for each test, and each player practiced several times before each test situation to familiarize himself with the action.

The instructions which were common to all three tests were: jump as high as possible and throw the ball as fast as possible with the intention of scoring a goal, using one hand and the correct throwing technique for a jump throw at goal. Take a maximum of three steps prior to the throw and throw from behind the free throw line, 9 m from the goal. All subjects were permitted to use resin on their hands thus simulating real playing actions. The test conditions were designed with the purpose of simulating real play.

All subjects had to perform a jump throw from 9 m from the goal under three different conditions (Figure 1): 1) without opposition where the throws were made without the presence of the goalkeeper or a defensive player; 2) with the opposition of the goalkeeper in which the throws were made in the presence of the goalkeeper, located on a line 0.5m from the goal and allowed only to make movements in the frontal plane with his hands or feet to try to intercept the ball; with an observer monitoring his action in each sequence of throws using a slow motion camera placed laterally to the goalkeeper and the trainers supervising performance to ensure that the players used the correct technique; and the last test condition was 3) where throws were made with opposition provided by the goalkeeper under the same conditions as in the previous situation, and a defensive player. The defensive player was situated on the 7 m line trying to intercept the ball with his hands and arms, was not allowed to move past the line and was told that if he tried to block the ball with a jump he was only allowed to jump vertically. An observer monitored his performance at all times. The accuracy criterion was the same in all the tests: throws should be accurate and aimed at the areas furthest from the goalkeeper, giving priority to the corners.

These experimental situation and conditions: a jump throw from a distance of 9 m with and without opposition from the goalkeeper and a defensive player were taken from the competition itself, and were representative of a real game situation (Gutiérrez et al., 2006).

Each participant carried out two rounds of throws. In the first round the order was with increasing degree of opposition (from without opposition to opposition with the goalkeeper and a defensive player). In the second round the reverse order was followed to guarantee that any possible differences were not due to the order of the tests or the fatigue caused by the accumulation of throws. This was confirmed in the subsequent data analysis. In each round the participant carried out jump throws until two recordings were made in each test, making four after performing two rounds. The two highest ball velocities for each test were used for further analysis.

The procedure followed in each series was as follows: each subject carried out a series of two throws with a pause of 10-15 seconds between them; if it was necessary to perform more throws because two results had not been recorded, the player carried out a second series of throws with a rest of 1-2 minutes between the series. The maximum number of series of throws allowed was two. A minimum of 3 minutes was given between tests to guarantee sufficient recovery for the player. (Figure 2) No player performed more than 15 throws in total, thus avoiding fatigue from the accumulation of explosive actions. In order to motivate the players they were informed of their ball velocity immediately after each attempt (Gorostiaga et al., 2005; Granados et al., 2007). The test lasted approximately 1h 30 min including the warm-up and the cool down.

Measurements

The tests were carried out on a team handball court in an indoor hall, with an official regulation ball weighing 480 g and with a circumference of 58 cm. Ball velocity was measured by radar (StalkerPro, Applied Concepts, Inc, Plano, USA), with a recording frequency of 100 Hz and with a sensitivity of 0.045 m·s-1, and the system was situated 15 m in front of the goal. To monitor the movements of the goalkeeper and defensive player a video camera was located laterally to both at a distance of 6m from the centre of the goal (Figure 1).

Statistical analysis

Means and standard deviations were presented for the each test for each group. A one-way ANOVA with repeated measures was used to analyze differences in ball velocity for each round of throws and in the anthropometrics, age and training experience between the groups.

To compare the effect of opposition and level of competition, a mixed design 3 (opposition: without, with only goalkeeper, and with goalkeeper and defence player: repeated measures) x 3 (group: elite, amateur, adolescent) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used with the Bonferroni post-hoc test. The statistical calculations were performed using the SPSS 14.0 program. The effect size and statistical power are presented in Tables 1 and 2">2. The effect size was evaluated with η2p (Eta partial squared) where 0.01<η2<0.06 constitutes a small effect, 0.06<η2<0.14 a medium effect and a large effect when η2>0.14 (Cohen, 1988). The intraclass correlation (ICC) and the coefficient of variation (CV) were calculated to ensure reliability; and were respectively 0.99 and 3.2% in the test without opposition, 0.98 and 3.8% in the test with the opposition of the goalkeeper and 0.96 and 4.7% in the test with the opposition of the goalkeeper and a defence player. The level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

RESULTS

Significant differences in age, training experience, height and body mass were found among the three groups i.e. the elite group was the oldest, tallest and had the most experience, followed by the amateur group and the under 18 group (Table 1). No significant differences were found in ball velocity between the first and second round (p > 0. 05). Therefore the best result of both rounds was used for further analysis.

A main effect of degree of opposition was found (Table 2). In general, ball velocity without opposition was 3.9% faster than throwing velocity with the opposition of the goalkeeper and 8.6% faster than the throwing velocity with the opposition of the goalkeeper and a defensive player (Figure 3, Table 2). Also the level of competition had an effect on ball velocity i.e. the elite group had the highest velocity, followed by the amateur groups and the lowest velocity was observed in the under 18 group (Figure 3, Table 2). However, no interaction was found between the levels of competition and the effect of opposition on ball velocity in the three different situations (Table 2).

DISCUSSION

In this study the influence of different degrees of opposition on ball velocity in the jump throw was examined in elite, amateur and adolescent players in team handball. It was found that the degree of opposition had a negative effect on ball velocity in all groups, and that the level of competition had a positive effect on ball velocity. However, no interaction was found between the level of competition and the degree of opposition on ball velocity.

The decreased throwing velocity with increasing degree of opposition is in line with the findings of Van der Wende, 2005 and Vila et al., 2009 who found significant differences in water polo between ball velocity with and without the opposition of the goalkeeper. In contrast, the results obtained by Gutiérrez et al., 2006 in team handball did not reveal differences between jump throw ball velocity with and without opposition, although kinematic differences were evident in the actions prior to the throw. Possible reasons for this discrepancy with the current study could be due to the level of team handball players (sub-elite) and the low number of participants (n = 11).

There was no interaction effect found from the level of competition and degree of opposition indicating that the degree of opposition had the same influence on all three groups in spite of their different experience in throwing. It was perhaps to be expected that the elite players would not be affected by the degree of opposition as found by Gutiérrez et al., 2006. However, they only compared throwing velocity with the opposition of a defence player and goalkeeper and not when there was only a goalkeeper.

The findings of the decreased throwing velocity with increasing degrees of opposition can be partly explained by the accuracy-velocity trade-off. It is generally accepted that a trade-off (guided information processing) exists between the accuracy and velocity at which a task is performed. The basis of this assumption lies in the work of Fitts, 1954. In team handball van den Tillaar and Ettema (2003a; 2003b; 2006) showed that when accuracy is more important, throwing velocity decreases which was also the case in our study. The presence of a goalkeeper and defence player increases the number of visual stimuli that have to be processed. According to Desimone and Duncan, 1995 in situations with multiple stimuli there is rivalry among them at the cognitive level to achieve the order in which they are processed and used to control behaviour. Subjectively, giving attention to one target leaves less available for others (Desimone and Duncan, 1995). In the case of throwing with opposition, visual stimuli related to accuracy could be prioritized reflecting on a decrease in attention to achieve maximum throwing velocity (Fitts, 1954). These increases in the number of visual stimuli to be processed can result in an adaptation of the throwing movement to the movements of the goalkeeper (and defensive player), e.g. to throw beside the block you have to change your throwing technique (change the movement of the trunk and throwing arm). Probably adjusting the throw to the opposition of the goalkeeper and defensive player may change the throwing kinematic pattern and could consequently change throwing velocity (Wagner et al. , 2010b). A few studies have investigated whether the inclusion of opposition causes changes in the technical execution of different sports skills. Gutiérrez et al., 2006 only found differences in the time of the run prior to the throw when opposition was involved, i.e. the time was shorter in the situation where there was opposition from a defence player compared to only opposition from a goalkeeper. No other kinematic differences were found in that study, indicating that the execution of the task was the same with and without opposition. Párraga et al., 2002 observed that the time of execution and accuracy did not differ with the position of the goalkeeper. However, both studies conducted 3D kinematic analysis at a frequency of only 50Hz, which probably could not detect eventual differences in kinematics at this level of execution. In other studies on the jump throw (Wagner et al., 2010a) a higher capturing resolution was used for identifying differences in throwing technique. A limitation of the current study is that we only measured ball velocity and no kinematic variables. Detailed 3D kinematic analysis of throwing with different degrees of opposition should be conducted to get a better understanding of changes in the execution of the throw under these conditions.

CONCLUSION

The degree of opposition had a negative effect upon ball velocity in elite, amateur and adolescent team handball players in the jump throw, indicating that an increase of external stimuli probably influences throwing kinematics and thereby maximal ball velocity. This influence of the degree of opposition upon throwing velocity was similar for all three groups, which indicates that experience does not seem to be a factor that can reduce the influence of these external stimuli. Detailed 3D kinematic analysis of throwing with these different degrees of opposition should be conducted to get a better understanding of changes in the execution of the throw under these conditions.

AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Jesús Rivilla-Garcia
Employment: Sports Department. Faculty of Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Physical conditioning, focusing on team sports, and high performance.
E-mail: esus.rivilla@upm.es
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ignacio Grande
Employment: Sports Department. Faculty of Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Physical conditioning, focusing on individual sports.
E-mail: ignacio.grande@upm.es
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Javier Sampedro
Employment: Sports Department. Faculty of Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Polytechnic University of Madrid.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Analysis performance sport team and recreation.
E-mail: javier.sampedro@upm.es
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Roland Van Den Tillaar
Employment: Reasearcher of the Research Centre for Sport, Health and Human Development, Vila Real, Portugal and at Department of Teacher Education and Sports of Sogn and Fjordane University College, Norway.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Coordination and mechanics of fast discrete complex movements (throwing) sprint training, biomechanics and physiology of strength training, whole-body vibration training.
E-mail: roland.tillaar@svt.ntnu.no
 
 
REFERENCES
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van der Wende K. (2005) Doctoral Thesis, Auckland University of Technology. The effects of game specific task constraints on the outcome of the water polo shot. New Zealand. Faculty of Health and Environmental Science.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Bayios I., Boudoulos K., Riehle H.J., Vieten M.M. (1998) Proceedings of the XVIth International Symposium on Biomechanics in Sports. Accuracy and throwing velocity in handball. Konstanz. ISBS.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Bayios I.A., Anastasopoulou E.M., Sioudris D.S., Boudolos K.D. (2001) Relationship between isokinetic strength of the internal and external shoulder rotators and ball velocity in team handball. Journal of Sports Medicine & Physical Fitness 441, 229-235.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Cohen J. (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd edition. New Jersey. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Desimone R., Duncan J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18, 193-222.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Fitts P.M. (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology 447, 381-391.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Fradet L., Botcazou M., Durocher C., Cretual A., Multon F., Prioux J., Delamarche P. (2004) Do handball throws always exhibit a proximal-to-distal segment sequence?. European Journal of Sport Science 22, 439-447.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Gorostiaga E.M., Granados C., Ibኈez J., Izquierdo M. (2005) Differences in physical fitness and throwing velocity among elite and amateur male handball players. International Journal of Sports Medicine 226, 225-232.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Granados C., Izquierdo M., Ibanez J., Bonnabau H., Gorostiaga E.M. (2007) Differences in physical fitness and throwing velocity among elite and amateur female handball players. International Journal of Sports Medicine 228, 860-867.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Gutiérrez M., Garcia P.L, Parraga J., Rojas F.J.. (2006) Effect of opposition on the handball jump shot. Journal of Human Movement Studies 51, 257-275.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Jöris H., Edwards V.M., Van Ingen Schenau G.J., Kemper H.C.G. (1985) Force, velocity and energy flow during the overarm throw in female handball players. Journal of Biomechanics 118, 409-414.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Marques M.C., Van den Tillaar R., Vescovi J.D., González-Badillo J.J. (2007) Relationship between throwing velocity, muscle power, and bar velocity during bench press in elite handball players. International Journal of Sports Physiology & Performance 22, 414-422.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Párraga J., Gutiérrez M., Rojas F.J., Ona A. (2002) The effects of visual stimuli on response reaction time and kinematic factors in the handball shot. Journal of Human Movement Studies 42, 421-439.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Pori P., Bon M., Sibila M. (2005) Jump shot performance in team handball. A kinematic model evaluated on the basis of expert modeling. International Journal of Fundamental and Applied Kinesiology 37, 40-49.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Singer R.N., Janelle C.M. (1999) Determining sport expertise: from genes to supremes. International Journal of Sport Psychology 30, 117-150.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van den Tillaar R., Ettema G. (2003a) Influence of instruction on velocity and accuracy of overarm throwing. Perceptual & Motor Skills 996, 423-434.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van den Tillaar R., Ettema G. (2003b) Instructions emphasizing velocity, accuracy, or both in performance and kinematics of overarm throwing by experienced team handball players. Perceptual & Motor Skills 97, 731-742.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van den Tillaar R., Ettema G. (2004) A force-velocity relationship and coordination patterns in overarm throwing. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine 3, 211-219.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van den Tillaar R., Ettema G. (2006) A comparison between novices and experts of the velocity-accuracy trade-off in overarm throwing. Perceptual & Motor Skills 103, 503-514.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van den Tillaar R., Ettema G. (2007) A three-dimensional analysis of overarm throwing in experienced handball players. Journal of Applied Biomechanics 23, 12-19.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Van Muijen A.E., Joris H., Kemper H.C., Van Ingen Schenau G.J. (1991) Throwing practice with different ball weights: Effects on throwing velocity and muscle strength in female handball players. Sports Training, Medicine & Rehabilitation 22, 103-113.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Vila H., Ferragut C., Argudo F.M., Abraldes J.A., Rodríguez N., Alacid F. (2009) Relationship between anthropometric parameters and throwing velocity in water polo players. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise 44, 62-74.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wagner H., Buchecker M., von Duvillard S. P., Müller E. (2010a) Kinematic description of elite vs. low level players in team-handball jump throw. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine 9, 15-23.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wagner H., Buchecker M., von Duvillard S. P., Müller E. (2010b) Kinematic comparison of team handball throwing with two different arm positions. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance 5, 469-483.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wagner H., Kainrath S., Müller E. (2008) Coordinative and tactical parameters of team-handball throw. The correlation of level of performance, throwing quality and selected technique-tactical parameters. Leistungssport 338, 35-41.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Šibila M., Pori P., Bon M. (2003) Basic kinematic differences between two types of jump shot techniques in handball. Universitatis Palackianae Olomucensis Gymnica 333, 19-26.
 
 
 
Home Issues About Authors
Contact Current Editorial board Authors instructions
Email alerts In Press Mission For Reviewers
Archive Scope
Supplements Statistics
Most Read Articles
  Most Cited Articles
 
  
 
JSSM | Copyright 2001-2024 | All rights reserved. | LEGAL NOTICES | Publisher

It is forbidden the total or partial reproduction of this web site and the published materials, the treatment of its database, any kind of transition and for any means, either electronic, mechanic or other methods, without the previous written permission of the JSSM.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.