Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
ISSN: 1303 - 2968   
Ios-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Views
7557
Download
5726
 
©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2022) 21, 23 - 32   DOI: https://doi.org/10.52082/jssm.2022.23

Research article
A Combination of Ice Ingestion and Head Cooling Enhances Cognitive Performance during Endurance Exercise in the Heat
Nur Shakila Mazalan1,3, , Grant Justin Landers1, Karen Elizabeth Wallman1, Ullrich Ecker2  
Author Information
1 School of Human Sciences (Exercise and Sports Science), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
2 School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
3 Faculty of Education, National University of Malaysia, Bangi, Selangor, Malaysia

Nur Shakila Mazalan
✉ School of Human Sciences (Exercise and Sports Science), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Email: nurshakila.mazalan@research.uwa.edu.au
Publish Date
Received: 29-07-2021
Accepted: 20-10-2021
Published (online): 15-02-2021
 
ABSTRACT

This study assessed the effectiveness of head cooling during exercise in the heat on cognitive performance, either alone or with ice ingestion. Ten healthy males, non-acclimatized to heat, ran (70% V̇O2peak) for 2×30 min in heat (35 ± 0.9°C, 68.2 ± 6.9% RH). Participants completed 3 trials: 10 min of head cooling during exercise (HC); precooling with crushed ice (7gikg-1) and head cooling during exercise (MIX); or no-cooling/control (CON). Working memory was assessed using the automated operation span task (OSPAN) and serial seven test (S7). Following MIX, S7 scores were improved compared to CON (12 ± 9.5, p = 0.004, d = 1.42, 0.34-2.28) and HC (4 ± 5.5, p = 0.048, d = 0.45, -0.47 to 1.3) during exercise. Moderate to large effect sizes were recorded for S7 and OSPAN following MIX and HC compared to CON, suggesting a tendency for improved cognitive performance during exercise in heat. Following precooling (MIX), core body temperature (Tc) and forehead temperature (Th) were lower compared to baseline (-0.75 ± 0.37°C, p < 0.001; -0.31 ± 0.29°C, p = 0.008, respectively) but not in HC or CON (p > 0.05). Thermal sensation (TS) was lower in MIX and HC compared to CON during exercise (p < 0.05). The reduction in Tc, Th and TS with MIX may have attenuated the effect of heat and subsequently improved working memory during exercise in heat.

Key words: Cooling, forehead temperature, cognitive function


           Key Points
  • The combination of crushed ice ingestion (precooling) and head cooling cap (percooling) significantly improved cognitive performance (working memory) on endurance exercise in the heat and this effect is attributed to the reduction in reduced core temperature, forehead temperature and thermal sensation.
  • Single cooling method during exercise via head cooling cap effective for attenuating impairment of working memory by providing maintenance during endurance exercise in the heat and this associated with reduced forehead temperature and thermal sensation following cooling.
  • Decrement in forehead temperature and thermal sensation with head cooling during exercise may benefit cognitive performance although with the absence of core body temperature.

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive function refers to mental processes that involve information processing, decision making and producing appropriate responses for problem solving (Purves, 2008) which occur in the frontal lobe of the brain (Buchbaum, 2004). Notably, sporting success requires cognitive function (perception, attention, and working memory) to operate optimally, particularly when exercising in the heat (Furley and Memmert, 2010). However, mental fatigue and reduction in cognitive capacity have been observed whilst exercising in thermally strain environment and these effects are associated with rise in core body temperature (Tc). During heat exposure and/or exercise, Tc increases, thus raising the temperature of blood flowing to the brain (Nybo et al., 2002). This is associated with the reduction in cerebral blood flow that could impair the delivery of substrates to the brain specifically oxygen and glucose and can potentially compromise brain function (Nybo et al., 2002). Notably, impairment in cognitive function has been reported when Tc ≥ 38.5°C (Gaoua et al., 2011; Racinais et al., 2008; Saldaris et al., 2020; Hocking et al., 2001).

Methods (external or internal) that cool the head/brain have been investigated with the aim of improving complex cognitive performance in heat mainly in working memory and decision making (Lee at al., 2014; Saldaris et al., 2019). Notably, Lee et al. (2014) reported that 5 min of necktie precooling prior to running in the heat (30°C, 70% relative humidity, RH), reduced neck temperature (~10.4°C, p ≤ 0.05) and improved cognitive performance (reaction time & executive control) assessed before and after exercise compared to control. Wearing a cooling cap represents another external method for cooling the head with only one study having assessed this method to date with no benefit on complex cognitive performance (working memory) found (Mazalan et al., 2021). Although cold water immersion has been suggested to be a gold standard in reducing heat strain, the cooling impact on muscle temperature and the administration whilst exercising need to be considered (Quod et al., 2006).

In regard to internal precooling, Saldaris et al. (2020) reported that precooling with crushed ice ingestion for 30 min (-0.1°C; 7g7kg-1) reduced thermal sensation (TS), Tc by -0.8°C and Th by -0.5°C compared to no cooling during running in the heat (35°C, 53% RH). Importantly, precooling maintained decision making (choice reaction time) and working memory (serial seven test [S7]) compared to no-cooling (Saldaris et al., 2020).

Further, combining internal and external cooling prior to exercise in the heat has also resulted in positive physiological outcomes. Levels et al. (2013), reported that precooling using a cooling cap (-10°C) combined with ice ingestion (2 g)kg-1) lowered Tc by ~0.6°C and reduced TS immediately after cooling but did not alter subsequent power output during a 15 km cycling time-trial in heat (30°C, 50% RH), compared to no-cooling. Cognitive performance and Th were not assessed in this study.

Another cooling technique growing in popularity is percooling, which refers to cooling parts of the body during exercise either continuously or intermittently. This method of cooling has resulted in a greater cooling effect on physiological (Ruddock et al., 2017; Tyler and Sunderland, 2011; Stevens et al. 2016) and cognitive performance assessed during rest and after exercise in the heat (Racinais et al., 2008; Saldaris et al., 2020) compared to precooling. Constant head and neck cooling during exercise has been found to sustain a lower Tc compared to no-cooling or precooling thus reducing thermal strain associated with exercise in the heat (Kenefick et al., 2007). Gaoua et al. (2011) reported that continuous head cooling whilst walking in the heat (50°C, 50% RH), decreased Tc by ~0.6°C, Th by ~1.9°C, and improved cognitive performance (spatial span test) assessed after exercise compared to a no-cooling (50°C, 50% RH). To date, no published studies have assessed the effect of intermittent percooling, using head cooling, on cognitive performance during exercise in the heat. This form of cooling may be preferable for athletes who are uncomfortable wearing a cooling cap for long periods of time.

The combination of precooling and percooling during exercise in the heat has been found to have greater physiological and psychological benefit than the use of either cooling modality alone (Best et al., 2018). Specifically, Hasegawa et al. (2006) reported that a combination of water immersion, 25°C (precooling: 30 min) and water ingestion, 14-16°C every 5 min (percooling) resulted in lower Tc and skin temperature (Tsk); longer exercise time to exhaustion; lower TS and rating of perceived exertion (RPE) compared to water ingestion alone (percooling) and no-cooling during cycling in heat (32°C, 80% RH). Cognitive performance was not assessed in this study.

Therefore, this study aimed to assess the effects of combining precooling (internal) and intermittent percooling (external) on cognitive performance assessed prior, during and after exercise in the heat. It was hypothesised that combining precooling via crushed ice ingestion with head cooling during exercise (percooling) would lower Tc and Th, leading to improved cognitive performance (specifically, working memory function, assessed with S7 and operation span [OSPAN] tasks) in the heat compared to intermittent percooling (head cooling) or no-cooling alone.

METHODS

Participants

Ten healthy males, non-acclimatized to heat (age 26.1 ± 1.9 y; height 1.75 ± 0.04 m; body-mass 76.8 ± 3.8 kg; V̇O2peak 53.89 ± 4.3 mL-1·kg-1·min), participated in this study. All participants reported previous training/running of ≥ 60 min performed ≥ four times per week. Ethical approval was granted (The University of Western Australia). Informed written consent were completed prior to participation in the study.

Preliminary procedures

In the 24 h prior to the first trial session, participants we asked to avoid strenuous exercise, alcohol, and caffeine. Upon arrival, body-mass (Model ED3300; Sauter Multi-Range, Ebingen, Germany) and height (Seca, Hamburg, Germany) were measured. Aerobic capacity was determined via a graded V̇O2peak test on a motorized treadmill (H/P Cosmos, Quasar 3p Medical treadmill, Nussdorf-Traunstein, Germany). Participants begun at 8 kmTh-1 (1% incline), increasing speed by 1 km h-1 every 3 min until volitional exhaustion (with 1 min recovery in between stages). Oxygen uptake (V̇O2) was measured continuously. Running speed during the subsequent trials was performed at a speed equivalent to 70% of each participant’s V̇O2peak. Participants were then familiarized with the cognitive tasks (OSPAN & S7), ingested crushed ice, wore the cooling cap and ran in the climate chamber (35°C, 70% RH) for 30 min.

Experimental designs

Three experimental trials were performed in a randomized order. All trials were conducted at the same time of day, seven days apart to control for circadian variability. Each trial involved running in the heat at 70% of V̇O2peak for 2×30 min bout with a 10 min break between periods. The interventions included: (1) sitting in the climate chamber for 30 min prior to running and head cooling applied during the last 10 min of each 30 min running period (HC: percooling); (2) crushed ice (7ghkg-1) ingested whilst sitting during the 30 min prior to the running protocol (precooling) and HC (MIX); (3) a no-cooling trial that involved consuming an equivalent amount of room temperature water while sitting for 30 min prior to running (CON).

Experimental protocol

Eight hours prior to each trial, participants ingested a telemetry capsule (CorTemp®, Palmetto, FL, USA) to enable measurement of gastrointestinal (core) temperature (Tc). On arrival, a mid-stream urine sample (1 ml) was collected to determine pre-exercise hydration status via urine specific gravity (USG) using a refractometer (TE-RM10SG, 1.000-1.070, Test Equip, Dandenong, Australia). In case of hypohydration (USG > 1.020; Volpe et al., 2009), participants then consumed 500 ml of water. Skin thermistors (Skin Sensor S7-1, Physitemp Instruments Inc, NJ, USA) were taped to the forehead (Th), sternum, left mid-anterior forearm and left mid-posterior calf. Mean skin temperature (Tsk) was calculated using the formula described by Burton (1935): Tsk = (0.5 × Tsternum) + (0.14 × Tforearm) + (0.36 × Tcalf).

Participants entered the climate chamber (35°C, 70% RH) and completed a baseline cognitive test (OSPAN) before ingesting either 7 g.kg-1 (-0.1°C) crushed ice (MIX) or water, ~22°C (HC and CON) in the subsequent 30 min whilst in a seated position. Participants then ran at 70% of V̇O2peak for 2 × 30 min periods on the treadmill with a 10 min break after the first 30 min. Head cooling (for HC and MIX) was performed whilst running during the last 10 min of each exercise bout. In CON, the cap was worn but not activated. The S7 was performed after the first minute of running during the first bout and then at the 27 min mark of both exercise bouts. The OSPAN was re-administered after the first and second bout of exercise. A 100 ml drink of water (22°C) was ingested every 10 min during exercise to prevent dehydration. Finally, Tc, Th, Tsk, HR, TS (0 = unbearably cold to 8 = unbearably hot; Young et al., 1987), and RPE (Borg scale 6-10; Borg, 1982) were measured every 5 min during exercise.

For MIX, participants ingested 7 g.kg-1 body-mass of crushed ice (-0.1°C) 30 min prior to exercise. The ice was ingested in three equal amounts at 0, 10, and 20 min. Commercially available headgear, Elasto-Gel Cranial Cap (Southwest Technologies, Inc, Missouri, USA) was used for HC and MIX (weight: 1.13 kg, thickness: 1.3 mm, dimensions: 31.8 × 22.6 mm). The device covered the whole head and neck areas and was secured around the neck with a velcro strap. Prior to testing, the headgear was kept in a freezer at -10°C.

The automated OSPAN task, a so-called complex span task used to assess working memory capacity (Turner and Engle, 1989), was performed 30 min prior to the running protocol, immediately after the first 30-min bout of running (during the 10 min break) and at the end of the exercise. The test was administered using Inquisit 5 software (Millisecond Software, Seattle, USA). Participants were required to memorize and then recall in order sequences of between 3 and 7 letters. Letters were presented individually at a rate of 1 item/s. During letter presentation, there was a distractor task: each letter was preceded by a simple math equation (e.g., 8 × 7 = 65) that participants had to assess as correct or incorrect. In total, there were 15 trials, and the outcome was measured by the total number of letters recalled correctly-in-position out of 75 (Bayliss et al., 2003). The task’s internal consistency is α = 0.78, while the test-retest reliability coefficient has been recorded to be r = 0.83 (Unsworth et al., 2005).

The serial seven test (S7) assesses concentration and working memory function (Bristow et al., 2016) and was administered after min 1 during the first 30-min bout of running (baseline) and again at the 27th min mark of the first and second 30-min bout of running. Here, participants were required to count down aloud, in sevens, from a random number provided to them (between 900 to 1000) for 1 min. The total number of correct answers and incorrect responses were recorded as outcome measures.

Statistical analyses

Data analysis was computed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences version 25.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). Data was assessed for normality (Shapiro-Wilk test) and sphericity (Mauchly’s test). Two-way repeated measures analyse of variance (ANOVA) with within-subject factors time (baseline vs. bout-1 and bout-2 measures at various intervals) and trials (CON; HC; MIX) were used. Statistical significance was accepted at p < 0.05. Where main interaction effects occurred, post hoc comparisons (Bonferroni) were conducted and if significance was found, paired sample t-tests were used to identify specific condition differences. In addition, Cohen’s d effect sizes (ES) were used to identify meaningful differences in the data (Cohen, 1988) with only moderate (0.5-0.8) and large (>0.8) ES reported. All values are expressed as mean ± SD.

RESULTS

There were no differences in temperature and humidity (35.5 ± 0.6°C, 69.3 ± 5.2% RH) in the environmental chamber between trials (all p > 0.05).

Cognitive tests

Serial seven test performance declined in CON trial but remained stable or even improved over time in the cooling trials. Accordingly, S7 scores yielded main effects of time (p = 0.007) and trials (p = 0.022), as well as a significant interaction (p < 0.001). A significant score reduction was found for CON after the first 27 min bout of exercise versus baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.21, -0.68 to 1.08 95% CI), with scores further declining after bout two of exercise versus baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.68, -0.27 to 1.52 95% CI). For MIX, S7 scores were greater after 27 min of exercise in both bouts relative to baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.63, -1.48 to 0.31 95% CI; p < 0.001, d = 0.47, -0.45 to 1.32 95% CI, respectively). We observed moderate to large ES after min 27 of the first bout of exercise between HC and CON (d = 0.66, 0.29 to1.50 95% CI) and MIX and CON (d = 1.09, 0.07 to 1.93 95% CI), with lower scores in the CON trial. Scores were numerically greater in HC after min 27 in the second bout of exercise compared to CON (d = 0.93, 0.09 to 1.77 95% CI). Finally, at the end of the second bout of exercise, more correct answers were recorded in MIX compared to CON (p = 0.004, d = 1.42, 0.34 to 2.28 95% CI) and HC (p = 0.048, d = 0.45, 0.47 to 1.30 95% CI; Table 1).

The same pattern emerged for the OSPAN scores. The ANOVA returned no significant main effect of trials (p = 0.27) but a main effect of time (p = 0.002) and a significant interaction effect (p = 0.001). A significant score reduction was found for CON after exercise (bout two) compared to baseline (p = 0.001, d = 1.54, 0.45 to 2.41 95% CI). A large ES was observed for MIX after min 30 of exercise (bout 1) compared to baseline with scores increasing over time (d = 0.89, 0.09 to 1.74 95% CI). Further, large ES were found for HC and MIX compared to CON (d = 1.12, 0.10 to 1.97 95% CI and d = 2.18, 0.93 to 3.09 95% CI, respectively) at min 30 (bout 1), with lower scores in CON. Finally, a large ES indicated a tendency for greater scores in HC and MIX trials compared to CON (d = 1.63, 0.51 to 2.49 95% CI; d = 2.26, -3.90 to -1.01 95% CI, respectively) after bout two of exercise; Table 1.

Core body temperature

There was an interaction effect for Tc (p < 0.001), and main effects for trials (p = 0.002) and time (p < 0.001). Core body temperature was similar at baseline (-30 min) for all trials (CON: 36.89 ± 0.38°C, HC: 36.84 ± 0.39°C, MIX: 36.98 ± 0.32°C, p = 0.618), but became lower in MIX (36.23 ± 0.31°C) at 20 min of precooling versus baseline (-0.75 ± 0.37°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.38, 1.09 to 3.31 95% CI). After precooling, MIX remained lower compared to CON (0.85 ± 0.29°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.65, 1.18 to 3.44 95% CI) and HC (0.79 ± 0.42°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.12, 0.89 to 3.03 95% CI).

After the first bout of exercise, MIX remained lower compared to CON and HC (MIX: 37.89 ± 0.35°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.65, 1.18 to 3.44 95% CI; HC: 38.30 ± 0.37°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.12, 0.89 to 3.03 95% CI). This was similar at the end of bout two of exercise where Tc was lower in MIX (39.16 ± 0.33°C) compared to CON (39.60 ± 0.30°C, p = 0.001, d = 11.41, 7.15 to 13.91 95% CI) and HC (39.58 ± 0.30°C, p = 0.01, d = 1.31, 0.26 to 2.16 95% CI; Figure 1).

Forehead temperature

Main effects for trials (p = 0.031) and time (P < 0.001) were found for Th. Forehead values were similar between trials at baseline (CON 35.61 ± 0.45°C; HC 35.45 ± 0.53°C; MIX 35.61 ± 0.33°C; p > 0.05). Following precooling, Th was lower in MIX by -0.31°C compared to baseline (p = 0.008, d = 0.81, 0.16 to1.65 95% CI) but not in HC (+0.15°C) and CON (+0.11°C) (p > 0.05). Following the first percooling during exercise, Th decreased by -0.48°C (MIX) and -0.47°C (HC) during min 25 versus min 20 (p = 0.001, d = 0.75, 0.21 to 1.6 95% CI; p = 0.012, d = 0.99, 0.01 to 1.83 95% CI, respectively). After exercise (bout 1), Th remained lower in MIX and HC compared to CON (p = 0.009, d = 1.53, 0.85 to 8.2 95% CI; p = 0.04, d = 1.06, 0.57 to 0.76 95% CI, respectively). Over the second percooling bout Th was lower by -0.54°C (MIX) and -0.58°C (HC) in min 25 compared to min 20 (p = 0.001, d = 1.1, 0.09 to 1.95 95% CI; p = 0.001, d = 1.2, 0.17 to 2.05 95% CI, respectively). At the end of exercise, Th was lower in MIX and HC versus CON (p < 0.001, d = 2.4, 1.1 to 3.33 95% CI and p = 0.002, d = 2.7, 1.3 to 3.6 95% CI, respectively; Figure 2).

Skin temperature

While Tsk increased in a similar pattern over time in all trials (p < 0.001, d: CON = 14.25, 8.98 to 17.32 95% CI; d: HC = 12.69, 7.97 to 15.43 95% CI; d: MIX = 14.28, 9 to 17.36 95% CI) (Figure 3), no differences were found between trials for any time point assessed (p > 0.05).

Thermal sensation

An interaction effect was found for TS (p = 0.001), while main effects were found for trials (p = 0.001) and time (P = 0.001; Table 2). Following the first percooling, TS decreased over time in HC and MIX, with results different to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 10.61, 6.63 to 12.95 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 7.5, 4.6 to 9.24 95% CI, respectively). Further, TS was lower in HC and MIX versus CON until the end of 30 min (bout 1) of exercise (p = 0.001, d = 5.88, 3.53 to 7.33 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 4.24, 2.42 to 5.41 95% CI).

During the second bout of percooling, TS was lower in HC and MIX compared to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 3.64, 2.0 to 4.72 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 3.16, 1.66 to 4.18 95% CI, respectively). Further, TS remained lower until the end of exercise (bout 2) for HC and MIX compared to CON (HC: p = 0.001, d=2.91, 1.48 to 3.9 95% CI; MIX: p = 0.001, d = 3.35, 1.8 to 4.39 95% CI).

Rating of perceived exertion

An interaction effect and main effect for time were found for RPE (p < 0.001; Table 3). There was no main effect for trials (p = 0.061). During exercise (bout 1), RPE was lower in HC and MIX compared to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 2.36, 1.07 to 3.28 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 4.71, 2.74 to 5.69 95% CI, respectively). Further, RPE values remained lower in MIX, but not in HC compared to CON at min 30 (p = 0.004, d = 1.99, 0.79 to 2.88 95% CI). Following percooling during exercise (bout 2), there were no differences between trials at any time points assessed (p > 0.05). Notably, during bout 2 of cooling, RPE values tended to be lower for HC compared to CON at min 25 (d = 1.0, 0.01 to 1.84 95% CI).

There were no differences in temperature and humidity (35.5 ± 0.6°C, 69.3 ± 5.2% RH) in the environmenttal chamber between trials (all p > 0.05).

Cognitive tests

Serial seven test performance declined in CON trial but remained stable or even improved over time in the cooling trials. Accordingly, S7 scores yielded main effects of time (p = 0.007) and trials (p = 0.022), as well as a significant interaction (p < 0.001). A significant score reduction was found for CON after the first 27 min bout of exercise versus baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.21, -0.68 to 1.08 95% CI), with scores further declining after bout two of exercise versus baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.68, -0.27 to 1.52 95% CI). For MIX, S7 scores were greater after 27 min of exercise in both bouts relative to baseline (p < 0.001, d = 0.63, -1.48 to 0.31 95% CI; p < 0.001, d = 0.47, -0.45 to 1.32 95% CI, respectively). We observed moderate to large ES after min 27 of the first bout of exercise between HC and CON (d = 0.66, 0.29 to1.50 95% CI) and MIX and CON (d = 1.09, 0.07 to 1.93 95% CI), with lower scores in the CON trial. Scores were numerically greater in HC after min 27 in the second bout of exercise compared to CON (d = 0.93, 0.09 to 1.77 95% CI). Finally, at the end of the second bout of exercise, more correct answers were recorded in MIX compared to CON (p = 0.004, d = 1.42, 0.34 to 2.28 95% CI) and HC (p = 0.048, d = 0.45, 0.47 to 1.30 95% CI; Table 1).

The same pattern emerged for the OSPAN scores. The ANOVA returned no significant main effect of trials (p = 0.27) but a main effect of time (p = 0.002) and a significant interaction effect (p = 0.001). A significant score reduction was found for CON after exercise (bout two) compared to baseline (p = 0.001, d = 1.54, 0.45 to 2.41 95% CI). A large ES was observed for MIX after min 30 of exercise (bout 1) compared to baseline with scores increasing over time (d = 0.89, 0.09 to 1.74 95% CI). Further, large ES were found for HC and MIX compared to CON (d = 1.12, 0.10 to 1.97 95% CI and d = 2.18, 0.93 to 3.09 95% CI, respectively) at min 30 (bout 1), with lower scores in CON. Finally, a large ES indicated a tendency for greater scores in HC and MIX trials compared to CON (d = 1.63, 0.51 to 2.49 95% CI; d = 2.26, -3.90 to -1.01 95% CI, respectively) after bout two of exercise; Table 1.

Core body temperature

There was an interaction effect for Tc (p < 0.001), and main effects for trials (p = 0.002) and time (p < 0.001). Core body temperature was similar at baseline (-30 min) for all trials (CON: 36.89 ± 0.38°C, HC: 36.84 ± 0.39°C, MIX: 36.98 ± 0.32°C, p = 0.618), but became lower in MIX (36.23 ± 0.31°C) at 20 min of precooling versus baseline (-0.75 ± 0.37°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.38, 1.09 to 3.31 95% CI). After precooling, MIX remained lower compared to CON (0.85 ± 0.29°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.65, 1.18 to 3.44 95% CI) and HC (0.79 ± 0.42°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.12, 0.89 to 3.03 95% CI).

After the first bout of exercise, MIX remained lower compared to CON and HC (MIX: 37.89 ± 0.35°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.65, 1.18 to 3.44 95% CI; HC: 38.30 ± 0.37°C, p < 0.001, d = 2.12, 0.89 to 3.03 95% CI). This was similar at the end of bout two of exercise where Tc was lower in MIX (39.16 ± 0.33°C) compared to CON (39.60 ± 0.30°C, p = 0.001, d = 11.41, 7.15 to 13.91 95% CI) and HC (39.58 ± 0.30°C, p = 0.01, d = 1.31, 0.26 to 2.16 95% CI; Figure 1).

Forehead temperature

Main effects for trials (p = 0.031) and time (p < 0.001) were found for Th. Forehead values were similar between trials at baseline (CON 35.61 ± 0.45°C; HC 35.45 ± 0.53°C; MIX 35.61 ± 0.33°C; p > 0.05). Following precooling, Th was lower in MIX by -0.31°C compared to baseline (p = 0.008, d = 0.81, 0.16 to1.65 95% CI) but not in HC (+0.15°C) and CON (+0.11°C) (p > 0.05). Following the first percooling during exercise, Th decreased by -0.48°C (MIX) and -0.47°C (HC) during min 25 versus min 20 (p = 0.001, d = 0.75, 0.21 to 1.6 95% CI; p = 0.012, d = 0.99, 0.01 to 1.83 95% CI, respectively). After exercise (bout 1), Th remained lower in MIX and HC compared to CON (p = 0.009, d = 1.53, 0.85 to 8.2 95% CI; p = 0.04, d = 1.06, 0.57 to 0.76 95% CI, respectively). Over the second percooling bout Th was lower by -0.54°C (MIX) and -0.58°C (HC) in min 25 compared to min 20 (p = 0.001, d = 1.1, 0.09 to 1.95 95% CI; p = 0.001, d = 1.2, 0.17 to 2.05 95% CI, respectively). At the end of exercise, Th was lower in MIX and HC versus CON (p < 0.001, d = 2.4, 1.1 to 3.33 95% CI and p = 0.002, d = 2.7, 1.3 to 3.6 95% CI, respectively; Figure 2).

Skin temperature

While Tsk increased in a similar pattern over time in all trials (p < 0.001, d: CON= 14.25, 8.98 to 17.32 95% CI; d: HC= 12.69, 7.97 to 15.43 95% CI; d: MIX= 14.28, 9 to 17.36 95% CI) (Figure 3), no differences were found between trials for any time point assessed (p > 0.05).

Thermal sensation

An interaction effect was found for TS (p = 0.001), while main effects were found for trials (p = 0.001) and time (p = 0.001; Table 2). Following the first percooling, TS decreased over time in HC and MIX, with results different to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 10.61, 6.63 to 12.95 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 7.5, 4.6 to 9.24 95% CI, respectively). Further, TS was lower in HC and MIX versus CON until the end of 30 min (bout 1) of exercise (p = 0.001, d = 5.88, 3.53 to 7.33 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 4.24, 2.42 to 5.41 95% CI).

During the second bout of percooling, TS was lower in HC and MIX compared to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 3.64, 2.0 to 4.72 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 3.16, 1.66 to 4.18 95% CI, respectively). Further, TS remained lower until the end of exercise (bout 2) for HC and MIX compared to CON (HC: p = 0.001, d = 2.91, 1.48 to 3.9 95% CI; MIX: p = 0.001, d = 3.35, 1.8 to 4.39 95% CI).

Rating of perceived exertion

An interaction effect and main effect for time were found for RPE (p < 0.001; Table 1). There was no main effect for trials (p = 0.061). During exercise (bout 1), RPE was lower in HC and MIX compared to CON at min 25 (p = 0.001, d = 2.36, 1.07 to 3.28 95% CI and p = 0.001, d = 4.71, 2.74 to 5.69 95% CI, respectively). Further, RPE values remained lower in MIX, but not in HC compared to CON at min 30 (p = 0.004, d = 1.99, 0.79 to 2.88 95% CI). Following percooling during exercise (bout 2), there were no differences between trials at any time points assessed (p > 0.05). Notably, during bout 2 of cooling, RPE values tended to be lower for HC compared to CON at min 25 (d = 1.0, 0.01 to 1.84 95% CI).

DISCUSSION

This study found that, in hot conditions, the combination of crushed ice (precooling) with head cooling (percooling) significantly improved S7 performance, meanwhile, single cooling method with HC maintained S7 over the duration of exercise compared to baseline respectively. Additionally, OSPAN scores in both cooling trials (MIX and HC) showed a tendency to be maintained over the same time compared to CON. Further, S7 and OSPAN scores were significantly impaired over time without a cooling intervention.

The impairment recorded for working memory over time in the CON trial was most likely due to rising Tc and Th as a result of exercise in the heat. Prolonged exercise in hot conditions will continuously store and accumulate heat in the body, which can disrupt the thermal equilibrium and diminish heat removal from the brain and body (Nybo et al., 2002). Previous studies have reported that a Tc ≥38.5°C has been found to be the onset of decrement on cognitive performance (Hocking et al., 2001; Schmit et al., 2017). Specifically, Hocking et al. (2001) reported that cognitive performances (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Digit Span and Inspection Time tasks) deteriorated when Tc increased above 38.5°C (peak: ~38.9°C) during exercise (walking at 5 km·h-1, incline 8-12%, for 40 min) in hot conditions (35°C, 65% RH) compared to a thermoneutral condition (25°C, 65% RH). In this study, forehead temperature was not measured. In the current study, working memory (S7 and OSPAN) was impaired in CON at a lower Tc ranging between 37.89°C and 38.30°C that occurred during the last five minutes of bout 1 of exercise. These values equated to a Th of 37.06 ± 0.51°C and 37.35 ± 0.51°C at the 25 and 30 min mark of bout 1 for CON. Conversely, S7 scores improved over time in both cooling trials compared to CON and baseline scores, despite Tc values peaking at 39.16 ± 0.33°C (MIX) and 39.58 ± 0.30°C (HC) at the end of the exercise protocol. Additionally, S7 scores were also significantly higher in MIX compared to HC at the end of bout 2 of exercise, with Tc being significantly lower in the MIX trial at this time point.

While increasing Tc values may have played some role in impairing S7 performance in HC at the end of bout 2 compared to MIX, the question arises as to why performance was significantly improved in the cooling trials compared to CON (and baseline) despite peak Tc values being greater than 38.5°C. It is possible that the most important factor in respect to S7 performance during exercise in the heat relates to Th, with this variable found to be lower in both cooling trials compared to CON at the end of both bouts of exercise, with peak Th values being 37.30 ± 0.47°C (MIX) and 37.51 ± 0.36°C (HC) compared to 38.54 ± 0.44°C (CON). This observation appears to also apply to OSPAN performance, where the tendency for better performance in both MIX and HC compared to CON, was associated with lower Th values at the end of both bouts of exercise compared to CON, while Tc was significantly lower in the MIX trial only (bout 1, 37.89 ± 0.35°C; and bout 2, 39.16 ± 0.33°C) when compared to CON. The higher Th values found for CON trial at the 25-30 min mark of bouts 1 and 2 compared to HC and MIX, may explain the impairment found in OSPAN scores for this trial compared to the cooling trials. These results suggest that factors associated with Th may play a role in working memory performance during and after exercise in the heat. These results are consistent with Gaoua et al. (2011) who reported improved working memory (spatial span test) performance in the heat following head cooling with ice packs compared to no-cooling, where Th was lower in the cooling trial (Th peak: ~38°C for cooling versus ~39.5°C for no-cooling).

Another consideration is that TS for MIX and HC was significantly lower than CON during the last 5 min of each bout of exercise, with RPE also being lower in either one or both cooling trials following both bouts of exercise compared to CON. Previous research reported significantly lower TS, along with Tc and Th, during 60 min of steady-state cycling (55% V̇O2peak) in the heat, compared to a control following crushed ice ingestion (Saldaris et al., 2019). In the current study, this reduction in TS could be related to the wearing of the cooling cap during exercise in the heat. Notably, the increased sensation of coolness following head cooling during exercise could mask the true state of the body (Tyler and Sunderland, 2011) even with Tc rising above 39°C by the end of the exercise protocol. This lower TS may have contributed to the improvement in cognitive performance, particularly during the final stages of exercise, despite Tc being ≥38.5°C; the critical value associated with cognitive fatigue.

In the current study, the combination of cooling removed heat from the body in several possible ways. Firstly, ice ingestion creates a heat sink in the body when ice is changed from a solid to liquid, with this process absorbing heat from the body (Seigel and Laursen, 2012). In the current study, this most likely led to significantly lower pre-exercise Tc and Th in MIX. Further, the significant reduction in Th seen at the start of exercise in MIX may have been due to the convective cooling of carotid blood inflow to the brain associated with swallowing crushed ice (Saldaris et al., 2019; Seigel and Laursen, 2012). Saldaris et al. (2020) reported lower Tc and Th following crushed ice in the heat compared to a control condition and found that precooling enabled participants to maintain decision making and working memory during exercise.

Further, head-cooling during exercise most likely removed heat away from the head region (skin, skull and brain) through conductive cooling. This process was found to lower Th following cooling in MIX and HC compared to CON. While head-cooling may have lowered Th, it did not reduce Tc due to similar Tc values recorded between the HC and CON trials over the course of the protocol. Further, the effect of head-cooling on MIX is speculative as there was no trial that assessed the effects of precooling with ice ingestion alone.

In the current study, despite the changes in Tc and Th associated with MIX, Tsk was not impacted, with results for this variable being similar between trials. Lack of change in Tsk between trials suggests that Tsk values that increase from ~34.7°C to 38.32°C have no obvious impact on S7 and OSPAN performance. This finding is supported by Simmons et al. (2008) who reported that Tsk was unlikely to influence/alter cognitive performance.

In this study, although S7 and OSPAN assessed complex cognitive tasks for working memory, the results differed from one another. The difference in the complexity level of each task (Hancock and Vasmatzidis, 2003) and the timing of the S7, which was completed whilst wearing the cooling cap (with participants feeling cooler as shown by TS scores), could be relevant factors. Further, the complexity of the OSPAN task required administration post rather than during exercise. As such, the impact of the cooling manipulation may have been stronger for the S7 task. Also, S7 is a single-verbal task and may provide a less specific but more sensitive measure, whereas OSPAN is a computer-based multifactorial task.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, this study demonstrated that while prolonged exercise in the heat impaired working memory, MIX resulted in significant improvement in S7 compared to HC and CON. Meanwhile, a tendency for S7 improvement was observed with single cooling alone via HC which indicate maintenance in cognitive function. Although OSPAN performance was not significantly improved with MIX and HC compared to CON, tendency of improvement in both trials were observed and this indicate that cooling may preserve working memory. This improvement in cognitive function in MIX most likely relates to a reduced Th associated with MIX and HC, compared to CON, that occurred after both bouts of exercise, with a lower Tc and TS also possibly contributing to this outcome.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to express their gratitude to the participants for their commitment and interest. The experiments comply with the current laws of the country in which they were performed. The authors have no conflict of interest to declare. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available, but are available from the corresponding author who was an organizer of the study.

AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Nur Shakila Mazalan
Employment: School of Human Sciences (Exercise and Sports Science), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Degree: PhD Student
Research interests: Exercise physiology, thermoregulation and cognitive performance
E-mail: nurshakila.mazalan@research.uwa.edu.au
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Grant Justin Landers
Employment: School of Human Sciences (Exercise and Sports Science), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Sport performance in different environments, kinanthropometry
E-mail: grant.landers@uwa.edu.au
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Karen Elizabeth Wallman
Employment: Associate Professor, School of Human Sciences (Exercise and Sports Science), The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Chronic fatigue syndrome, exercise physiology and biochemistry, sport and physical activity and obesity
E-mail: Karen.wallman@uwa.edu.au
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ullrich Ecker
Employment: Associate Professor, School of Psychological Science, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Human memory and reasoning
E-mail: ullrich.ecker@uwa.edu.au
 
 
REFERENCES
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Bayliss D. M., Jarrold C., Gunn D. M., Baddeley A. D. (2003) The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology General 132, 71-92.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Best R., Payton S., Spears I., Riera F., Berger N. (2018) Topical and ingested cooling methodologies for endurance exercise performance in the heat. Sports (Basel, Switzerland) 6, 11.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Borg G. A. (1982) Psychological bases of physical exertion. Medicine Science and Sports and Exercise 14, 377-381.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Bristow T., Jih C. S., Slabich A., Gunn J. (2016) Standardization and adult norms for the sequential tasks of serial 3’s and 7’s. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 23, 372-378.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Buchbaum M (2004) Images in Neuroscience: Frontal Cortex Function. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 2178.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Burton A (1935) Human calorimetry II. The average temperature of the tissues of the body. The Journal of Nutrition 9, 261-280.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Furley P. A., Memmert D. (2010) The role of working memory in sport. International Review of Sport Exercise and Psychology 3, 171-194.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Gaoua N., Racinais S., Grantham J., El M. F. (2011) Alterations in cognitive performance during passive hyperthermia are task dependent. International Journal of Hyperthermia 27, 1-9.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Hancock P., Vasmatzidis I. (2003) Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: the current state of knowledge. International Journal of Hyperthermia 19, 355-372.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Hasegawa H., Takatori T., Komura T., Yamasaki M. (2006) Combined effects of precooling and water ingestion in thermoregulation and physical capacity during exercise in a hot environment. Journal of Sports Science 42, 3-9.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Hocking C., Silberstein R. B., Lau W. M., Stough C., Roberts W. (2001) Evaluation of cognitive performance in the heat by functional brain imaging and psychometric testing. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A, Molecular and Integrative Physiology 128, 719-734.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Kenefick R.W., Cheuvront S. N., Sawka M. N. (2007) Thermoregulatory function during the marathon. Sports Medicine 37, 312-315.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Lee J. K., Koh A. C., Koh S. X., Liu G. J., Nio A. Q., Fan P. W. (2014) Neck cooling and cognitive performance following exercise-induced hyperthermia. European Journal of Applied Physiology 114, 375-384.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Levels K., Teunissen L. P., De H. A., De K. J., Van O. B., Daanen H. A. (2013) Effect of warm-up and precooling on pacing during a 15-km cycling time trial in the heat. International Journal of Sport Physiology and Performance 8, 307-311.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Mazalan N. S., Landers G. J., Wallman K. E., Ecker U. K. H. (2021) Head cooling prior to exercise in the heat does not improve cognitive performance. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine 20, 69-76.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Norton K., Coombes J., Parker R., Williams A., Hobson-Powell A., Knox C. (2012) New Australian standard for adult pre-exercise screening. Sport Health 30, 14-16.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Nybo L., Moller K., Volianitis S., Nielsen B., Secher N. H. (2002) Effect of hyperthermia on cerebral blood flow and metabolism during prolonged exercise in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology 93, 58-64.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Purves, D. (2008) Principles of cognitive neuroscience. Sunderland, Mass: Sinauer Associates.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Quod M. J., Martin D. T., Laursen P. B. (2006) Cooling athletes before competition in the heat: comparison of techniques and practical considerations. Sports Medicine 36, 671-682.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Racinais S., Gaoua N., Grantham J. (2008) Hyperthermia impairs short-term memory and peripheral motor drive transmission. Journal of Physiology 586, 4751-4762.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ruddock A., Robbins B., Tew G., Bourke L., Purvis A. (2017) Practical cooling strategies during continuous exercise in hot environments: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Medicine 47, 517-532.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Saldaris J. M., Landers G. J., Lay B. S. (2019) Physical and perceptual cooling: Improving cognitive function, mood disturbance and time to fatigue in the heat. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports 30, 801-811.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Saldaris J. M., Landers G. J., Lay B. S. (2020) Enhanced decision making and working memory during exercise in the heat with crushed ice ingestion. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance 15, 1-8.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Schmit C., Hausswirth C., Le M. Y., Duffield R. (2017) Cognitive functioning and heat strain: Performance responses and protective strategies. Journal of Sports Medicine 47, 1289-1302.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Seigel R., Laursen P. B. (2012) Keeping your body cool heat with internal cooling methods. Sports Medicine 42, 89-98.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Simmons S. E., Saxby B. K., McGlone F. P., Jones D. A. (2008) The effect of passive heating and head cooling on perception, cardiovascular function and cognitive performance in the heat. European Journal of Applied Physiology 104, 127-280.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Stevens C.J., Thoseby B., Sculley D. V., Callister R., Taylor L., Dascombe B. J. (2016) Running performance and thermal sensation in the heat are improved with menthol mouth rinse but not ice slurry ingestion. Scandinavian Journal Medicine and Science in Sport 26, 1209-1216.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Turner M.L., Engle R. W. (1989) Is working memory capacity task dependent?. Journal of Memory and Language 28, 127-154.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Tyler C. J., Sunderland C. (2011) Cooling the neck region during exercise in the heat. Journal of Athletic Training 46, 61-68.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Unsworth N., Heitz R.P., Schrock J.C., Engle R.W. (2005) An automated version of the operation span task. Behavioral Research Methods 37, 498-505.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Volpe S.L., Poule K. A., Bland E. G. (2009) Estimation of prepractice hydration status of National Collegiate Athletic Association Division I athletes. Journal of Athletic Training 44, 624-629.  Crossref
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Young A. J., Sawka M. N., Epstein Y., Decristofano B., Pandolf K. B. (1987) Cooling different body surfaces during upper and lower body exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology 63, 1218-1223.  Crossref
 
 
 
Home Issues About Authors
Contact Current Editorial board Authors instructions
Email alerts In Press Mission For Reviewers
Archive Scope
Supplements Statistics
Most Read Articles
  Most Cited Articles
 
  
 
JSSM | Copyright 2001-2024 | All rights reserved. | LEGAL NOTICES | Publisher

It is forbidden the total or partial reproduction of this web site and the published materials, the treatment of its database, any kind of transition and for any means, either electronic, mechanic or other methods, without the previous written permission of the JSSM.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.